排序方式: 共有174条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
121.
There is little experimental evidence that any non-human species is capable of purposefully attempting to manipulate the psychological states of others deceptively (e.g., manipulating what another sees). We show here that chimpanzees, one of humans' two closest primate relatives, sometimes attempt to actively conceal things from others. Specifically, when competing with a human in three novel tests, eight chimpanzees, from their first trials, chose to approach a contested food item via a route hidden from the human's view (sometimes using a circuitous path to do so). These findings not only corroborate previous work showing that chimpanzees know what others can and cannot see, but also suggest that when competing for food chimpanzees are skillful at manipulating, to their own advantage, whether others can or cannot see them. 相似文献
122.
Dogs can use the placement of an arbitrary marker to locate hidden food in an object-choice situation. We tested domestic
dogs (Canis familiaris) in three studies aimed at pinning down the relative contributions of the human’s hand and the marker itself. We baited one
of two cups (outside of the dogs’ view) and gave the dog a communicative cue to find the food. Study 1 systematically varied
dogs’ perceptual access to the marker placing event, so that dogs saw either the whole human, the hand only, the marker only,
or nothing. Follow-up trials investigated the effect of removing the marker before the dog’s choice. Dogs used the marker
as a communicative cue even when it had been removed prior to the dog’s choice and attached more importance to this cue than
to the hand that placed it although the presence of the hand boosted performance when it appeared together with the marker.
Study 2 directly contrasted the importance of the hand and the marker and revealed that the effect of the marker diminished
if it had been associated with both cups. In contrast touching both cups with the hand had no effect on performance. Study
3 investigated whether the means of marker placement (intentional or accidental) had an effect on dogs’ choices. Results showed
that dogs did not differentiate intentional and accidental placing of the marker. These results suggest that dogs use the
marker as a genuine communicative cue quite independently from the experimenter’s actions. 相似文献
123.
124.
Kremin H Akhutina T Basso A Davidoff J De Wilde M Kitzing P Lorenz A Perrier D van der Sandt-Koenderman M Vendrell J Weniger D Apt P Arabia C De Bleser R Cohen H Corbineau M Dolivet MC Hirsh K Lehoux E Metz-Lutz MN Montañes P Plagne S Polonskaya N Sirois M Stachowiak F Sweeney T Vish-Brink E 《Brain and cognition》2003,53(2):243-246
The well established effect of word frequency on adult's picture naming performance is now called into question. This is particularly true for variables which are correlated with frequency, as is the case of age of word acquisition. Since the work of [Carrol and White, 1973] there is growing agreement among researchers to confer an important role in lexical access to this variable. Indeed, it has been shown ( [Hodgson and Ellis, 1998]) that for normal English-speaking adults only the variables 'age-of-acquisition' and 'name agreement' are independent predictors of naming success among the various variables considered. However, when brain-damaged subjects with and without degenerative pathologies are studied, word frequency and word length as well as concept familiarity all give significant effects ( [Hirsh and Funnell, 1995]; [Lambon Ralph et al., 1998]; [Nickels and Howard, 1995]). Finally, it has been suggested that the production of specific error types may be related to such variables. According to [Nickels and Howard, 1994] the production of semantic errors is specifically affected by 'imageability' and in the recent study by [Kremin et al., 2001] 'age of acquisition' predicts (frank) word finding difficulties. 相似文献
125.
Marco Bennardi Francisco Flix Caballero Marta Miret Jose Luis Ayuso‐Mateos Josep Maria Haro Elvira Lara Ella Arensman Maria Cabello 《Suicide & life-threatening behavior》2019,49(1):90-103
The aims of this study were to analyze whether positive and negative affect, social support, and loneliness are factors longitudinally related to suicide ideation in the general population in different age groups. A total of 2,392 individuals from a nationally representative sample of the Spanish general population were evaluated in 2011–2012 and in 2014–2015. After including relevant control variables in the analyses, lower positive affect was prospectively related to ideation in 18‐ to 59‐year‐old individuals, whereas feelings of loneliness were related to ideation in 60‐year‐and‐older individuals. Social support was not associated with suicide ideation in any age group. These results are in line with the need for age‐tailored suicide prevention programs. The present findings might also suggest that health care professionals should consider feelings of loneliness rather than social support to assess the presence of suicide ideation in older people. 相似文献
126.
Josep Demestre Sheila Meltzer José E. García-Albea Andreu Vigil 《Journal of psycholinguistic research》1999,28(3):293-312
Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) were recorded during spoken language comprehension to study the on-line effects of gender agreement violations in controlled infinitival complements. Spanish sentences were constructed in which the complement clause contained a predicate adjective marked for syntactic gender. By manipulating the gender of the antecedent (i.e., the controller) of the implicit subject while holding constant the gender of the adjective, pairs of grammatical and ungrammatical sentences were created. The detection of such a gender agreement violation would indicate that the parser had established the coreference relation between the null subject and its antecedent. The results showed a complex biphasic ERP (i.e., an early negativity with prominence at anterior and central sites, followed by a centroparietal positivity) in the violating condition as compared to the non-violating conditions. The brain reacts to NP-adjective gender agreement violations within a few hundred milliseconds of their occurrence. The data imply that the parser has properly coindexed the null subject of an infinitive clause with its antecedent. 相似文献
127.
Blanca Mellor-Marsá Marta Miret Francisco J. Abad Somnath Chatterji Beatriz Olaya Beata Tobiasz-Adamczyk Seppo Koskinen Matilde Leonardi Josep Maria Haro José Luis Ayuso-Mateos Francisco Félix Caballero 《Journal of Happiness Studies》2016,17(5):1769-1787
Given the growing interest in the study of subjective well-being as a measure of social progress, instruments that produce valid and reliable scores and that can be used within and across countries are needed. The aim of the present study was to analyze the measurement equivalence of the Day Reconstruction Method in its brief version, using nationally representative samples from Finland, Poland, and Spain obtained within the COURAGE in Europe project. The goodness-of-fit of a two-correlated-factors model and the reliability of the scores obtained were assessed. Cross-country invariance was tested employing a multiple group confirmatory factor analysis, through sequential constraint imposition. In each country, measurement invariance was tested across time frames (morning, afternoon and evening) and days of the week (weekday and weekend). The results found support for the hypothesis of a two-correlated-factors (positive and negative affect) structure; the reliability of the positive, the negative and the net affect scores showed appropriate values. A high equivalence across the three national samples was found: all items except one showed strong measurement invariance indicating that respondents from Finland, Poland, and Spain attribute the same meaning to the latent construct under study, and the levels of the underlying items are equal in all three countries. Similar results were found for the measurement equivalence across time frames and days of the week. Our findings support the assumption of comparability across the different samples considered; in general, higher positive affect and lower negative affect were found in Finland, in the evening and at the weekend. 相似文献
128.
129.
Josep Call 《Animal cognition》2010,13(5):689-700
When confronted with uncertain or incomplete information in decision-making situations, monkeys and apes opt for either escaping
the situation or seeking additional information. These responses have been interpreted as evidence of metacognitive abilities.
However, this interpretation has been challenged. On the one hand, studies using the information-seeking paradigm have been
criticized because subjects may simply engage in a search for information routine (e.g., search until spot the reward) without
any metacognitive involvement. On the other hand, studies using the escape response paradigm have been criticized because
subjects may not recognize their own state of uncertainty but have learned to use the escape response in the presence of certain
stimuli configurations that create uncertainty. The current study attempted to address these two criticisms by presenting
great apes (seven gorillas, eight chimpanzees, four bonobos, seven orangutans) with a seeking information task whose basic
procedure consisted of presenting two hollow tubes, baiting one of them and letting subjects choose. Conditions varied depending
on whether subjects had visual access to the baiting, the cost associated with seeking information, the time interval between
baiting and choosing, the food quality and the additional information offered regarding the food’s location. Although subjects
showed a high retrieval accuracy when they had witnessed the baiting, they were more likely to check inside the tube before
choosing when high stakes were involved (Experiment 3) or after a longer period of time had elapsed between the baiting and
the retrieval of the reward (Experiment 2). In contrast, providing subjects with indirect auditory information about the food’s
location or increasing the cost of checking reduced checking before choosing (Experiment 1). Taken together, these findings
suggest that subjects knew that they could be wrong when choosing. 相似文献
130.
Recognizing relational similarity relies on the ability to understand that defining object properties might not lie in the objects individually, but in the relations of the properties of various object to each other. This aptitude is highly relevant for many important human skills such as language, reasoning, categorization and understanding analogy and metaphor. In the current study, we investigated the ability to recognize relational similarities by testing five species of great apes, including human children in a spatial task. We found that all species performed better if related elements are connected by logico-causal as opposed to non-causal relations. Further, we find that only children above 4 years of age, bonobos and chimpanzees, unlike younger children, gorillas and orangutans display some mastery of reasoning by non-causal relational similarity. We conclude that recognizing relational similarity is not in its entirety unique to the human species. The lack of a capability for language does not prohibit recognition of simple relational similarities. The data are discussed in the light of the phylogenetic tree of relatedness of the great apes. 相似文献