全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2727篇 |
免费 | 98篇 |
国内免费 | 23篇 |
专业分类
2848篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 40篇 |
2023年 | 43篇 |
2022年 | 52篇 |
2021年 | 90篇 |
2020年 | 89篇 |
2019年 | 92篇 |
2018年 | 155篇 |
2017年 | 125篇 |
2016年 | 117篇 |
2015年 | 75篇 |
2014年 | 94篇 |
2013年 | 334篇 |
2012年 | 187篇 |
2011年 | 182篇 |
2010年 | 136篇 |
2009年 | 68篇 |
2008年 | 167篇 |
2007年 | 123篇 |
2006年 | 124篇 |
2005年 | 83篇 |
2004年 | 78篇 |
2003年 | 42篇 |
2002年 | 49篇 |
2001年 | 23篇 |
2000年 | 30篇 |
1999年 | 24篇 |
1998年 | 16篇 |
1997年 | 10篇 |
1996年 | 7篇 |
1995年 | 6篇 |
1994年 | 10篇 |
1993年 | 9篇 |
1992年 | 17篇 |
1991年 | 6篇 |
1990年 | 8篇 |
1989年 | 12篇 |
1988年 | 7篇 |
1987年 | 12篇 |
1986年 | 10篇 |
1985年 | 5篇 |
1983年 | 6篇 |
1981年 | 7篇 |
1975年 | 4篇 |
1974年 | 4篇 |
1973年 | 8篇 |
1971年 | 8篇 |
1968年 | 10篇 |
1966年 | 4篇 |
1965年 | 4篇 |
1939年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有2848条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
Wenchao Ma Jimmy de la Torre 《The British journal of mathematical and statistical psychology》2020,73(1):142-163
As a core component of most cognitive diagnosis models, the Q-matrix, or item and attribute association matrix, is typically developed by domain experts, and tends to be subjective. It is critical to validate the Q-matrix empirically because a misspecified Q-matrix could result in erroneous attribute estimation. Most existing Q-matrix validation procedures are developed for dichotomous responses. However, in this paper, we propose a method to empirically detect and correct the misspecifications in the Q-matrix for graded response data based on the sequential generalized deterministic inputs, noisy ‘and’ gate (G-DINA) model. The proposed Q-matrix validation procedure is implemented in a stepwise manner based on the Wald test and an effect size measure. The feasibility of the proposed method is examined using simulation studies. Also, a set of data from the Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study (TIMSS) 2011 mathematics assessment is analysed for illustration. 相似文献
52.
Viktor J. Tóth 《Dialog》2020,59(4):352-355
This article focuses on the third volume of Vel-Matti Kärkkäinen's systematic theology, titled Creation and Humanity. After a brief overview of the volume the article offers some clarification on the tenth chapter, especially on its interaction with paleontology. After that, two constructive observations follow. The first is related to Kärkkäinen's notion of the embeddedness of human nature, and the second is to further employ systems thinking in his constructive work on creation and human evolution. 相似文献
53.
Recently, cross-cultural facial-expression recognition has become a research hotspot, and a standardised facial-expression material system can significantly help researchers compare and demonstrate the results of other studies. We developed a facial-expression database of Chinese Han, Hui and Tibetan ethnicities. In this study, six basic human facial expressions (and one neutral expression) were collected from 200 Han, 220 Hui and 210 Tibetan participants who lived in these regions. Four experts on each ethnicity evaluated the facial-expression images according to the expressions, and only those achieving inter-rater agreement were retained. Subsequently, 240 raters evaluated these images according to the seven emotions and rated the intensity of the expressions. Consequently, 2980 images were included in the database, including 930 images of Han individuals, 962 images of Hui individuals and 1088 images of Tibetan individuals. In conclusion, the facial-expression database of Chinese Han, Hui and Tibetan people was representative and reliable with a recognition rate of over 60%, making it well-suited for cross-cultural research on emotions. 相似文献
54.
Cristina Senín-Calderón Jenifer Gálvez-González Salvador Perona-Garcelán Carlos Camacho Juan F. Rodríguez-Testal 《International journal of psychology》2020,55(5):832-841
This study examined a moderated mediation model testing whether dysmorphic concern is related to behaviour altered to change appearance. This relationship is potentially mediated by depressive symptomatology (dysphoria and self-esteem) and ideas of reference about “laughing, commenting” and “attention, appearance,” and each mediated relationship moderated by sex. The sample was made up of 3377 adolescents from 12 to 18 years old (Mage = 14.52; SD = 1.65, 56.5% girls). The results showed that dysphoria and ideas of reference about “laughing, commenting” and “attention, appearance” partially mediated the relationship between dysmorphic concern and behavioural impairment related to body image. The relationship with dysphoria was moderated by sex, such that the mediation effect was stronger in girls than in boys. This result implies that girls who are worried about some characteristic of their appearance and show dysphoria are at greater risk of altered behaviour involving avoidance or controlling their appearance than boys. In addition, a possible risk of body dysmorphic disorder (3.45% of the sample) was found, with very prominent hiding behaviour using clothing or control behaviours, such as frequent weighing and looking at oneself in the mirror too much. Implications of these findings are discussed. 相似文献
55.
56.
Atypical depression: enhanced right hemispheric dominance for perceiving emotional chimeric faces 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Bruder GE Stewart JW McGrath PJ Ma GJ Wexler BE Quitkin FM 《Journal of abnormal psychology》2002,111(3):446-454
Two studies compared hemispatial bias for perceiving chimeric faces in patients having either atypical or typical depression and healthy controls. A total of 245 patients having major depressive disorder (MDD) or dysthymia (164 with atypical features) and 115 controls were tested on the Chimeric Faces Test. Atypical depression differed from typical depression and controls in showing abnormally large right hemisphere bias. This was present in patients having either MDD or dysthymia and was not related to anxiety, physical anhedonia, or vegetative symptoms. In contrast, patients having MDD with melancholia showed essentially no right hemisphere bias. This is further evidence that atypical depression is a biologically distinct subtype and underscores the importance of this diagnostic distinction for neurophysiologic studies. 相似文献
57.
58.
The contextual modulation of conditioned taste aversions by the physical environment and time of day is similar
下载免费PDF全文

Morón I Manrique T Molero A Ballesteros MA Gallo M Fenton A 《Learning & memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.)》2002,9(5):218-223
In a pair of experiments, we have compared the ability of changes of place (Exp. 1) and changes of time of day (Exp. 2) to separately modulate learned saline-aversion memory phenomena in rats. Neither a spatial nor a temporal change disrupted latent inhibition using the present behavioral procedure. However, pre-exposure to the taste increased the contextual control of the learned aversion expression. The aversion reappeared in the place or at the time of conditioning after extinction in a different context. The results indicate that environmental and temporal contexts can independently, but similarly modulate taste aversion learning. 相似文献
59.
Combinatory logic is known to be related to substructural logics. Algebraic considerations of the latter, in particular, algebraic considerations of two distinct implications (, ), led to the introduction of dual combinators in Dunn & Meyer 1997. Dual combinators are "mirror images" of the usual combinators and as such do not constitute an interesting subject of investigation by themselves. However, when combined with the usual combinators (e.g., in order to recover associativity in a sequent calculus), the whole system exhibits new features. A dual combinatory system with weak equality typically lacks the Church-Rosser property, and in general it is inconsistent. In many subsystems terms "unexpectedly" turn out to be weakly equivalent. The paper is a preliminary attempt to investigate some of these issues, as well as, briefly compare function application in symmetric -calculus (cf. Barbanera & Berardi 1996) and dual combinatory logic. 相似文献
60.
Attention and joint attention in preschool children with autism 总被引:13,自引:0,他引:13
Three experiments examined the role of attention in explaining dyadic (child-adult) and triadic (child-adult-object) joint attention difficulties in autism. Experiments 1 and 2 investigated children's ability to orient to an adult's attention bid and to follow the direction of a human or nonhuman cue. Experiment 3 tested ability to disengage and shift attention to objects. Results showed autism-specific difficulties at both dyadic and triadic levels. Children with autism were less responsive than developmentally delayed controls in orienting to attention bids and in following a human head-turn cue yet had no difficulty in shifting attention and were faster overall in orienting to targets. Results suggest a specific developmental delay in which children with autism rely on the presence of objects in the visual field to guide action. The relation between this problem and autistic children's difficulties with human communicative signals is discussed. 相似文献