首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1868篇
  免费   99篇
  国内免费   1篇
  1968篇
  2024年   3篇
  2023年   23篇
  2022年   8篇
  2021年   32篇
  2020年   38篇
  2019年   30篇
  2018年   81篇
  2017年   74篇
  2016年   69篇
  2015年   46篇
  2014年   64篇
  2013年   304篇
  2012年   151篇
  2011年   139篇
  2010年   75篇
  2009年   54篇
  2008年   131篇
  2007年   118篇
  2006年   91篇
  2005年   62篇
  2004年   37篇
  2003年   42篇
  2002年   43篇
  2001年   10篇
  2000年   29篇
  1999年   19篇
  1998年   17篇
  1997年   11篇
  1996年   15篇
  1995年   9篇
  1994年   19篇
  1993年   5篇
  1992年   6篇
  1991年   16篇
  1990年   8篇
  1989年   10篇
  1988年   6篇
  1987年   5篇
  1986年   3篇
  1985年   3篇
  1984年   10篇
  1983年   5篇
  1982年   3篇
  1980年   3篇
  1979年   5篇
  1977年   4篇
  1975年   4篇
  1974年   3篇
  1973年   3篇
  1946年   2篇
排序方式: 共有1968条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
251.
252.
The perception of face gender was examined in the context of extending “face space” models of human face representations to include the perceptual categories defined by male and female faces. We collected data on the recognizability, gender classifiability (reaction time to classify a face as male/female), attractiveness, and masculinity/femininity of individual male and female faces. Factor analyses applied separately to the data for male and female faces yielded the following results. First, for both male and female faces, the recognizability and gender classifiability of faces were independent—a result inconsistent with the hypothesis that both recognizability and gender classifiability depend on a face’s “distance” from the subcategory gender prototype. Instead, caricatured aspects of gender (femininity/masculinity ratings) related to the gender classifiability of the faces. Second, facial attractiveness related inversely to face recognizability for male, but not for female, faces—a result that resolves inconsistencies in previous studies. Third, attractiveness and femininity for female faces were nearly equivalent, but attractiveness and masculinity for male faces were not equivalent. Finally, we applied principal component analysis to the pixel-coded face images with the aim of extracting measures related to the gender classifiability and recognizability of individual faces. We incorporated these model-derived measures into the factor analysis with the human rating and performance measures.  相似文献   
253.
The present study examined whether the cognitive bias for threat is a stable phenomenon in spider phobics and not in nonphobic controls. The rationale of this study was that the use of emotional bias for individual assessment in clinical practice requires stability of this phenomenon. In order to assess the stability of the emotional bias, the spider Stroop task was administered twice to spider phobics and controls, with a time lag of 3 weeks. In two experiments, spider phobics (nexp. 1 = 20; nexp. 2 = 33) and controls (nexp. 1 = 24; nexp. 2 = 25) were selected on basis of a structured interview. In the second experiment, subjects anticipated exposure to a real-life spider. Emotional bias was inferred from color-naming latencies on spider words versus control words. In line with our prediction, the bias for threat was stable in spider phobic individuals but not in controls. This stability could not be attributed to a general cognitive characteristic because a standard Stroop task did not differentiate between the spider phobics and the controls. The results support the view that cognitive processing of threat in anxiety is stable. It is proposed to use both the emotional bias and its stability as measures of the constraints on the flexibility of the cognitive fear-network.  相似文献   
254.
The nature of psychological measurements in relation to mathematical structures and representations is examined. Some very general notions concerning algebras and systems are introduced and applied to physical and number systems, and to measurement theory. It is shown that the classical intensive and extensive dimensions of measurements with their respective ordinal and additive scales are not adequate to describe physical events without the introduction of the notions of dimensional units and of dimensional homogeneity. It is also shown that in the absence of these notions, the resulting systems of magnitudes have only a very restricted kind of isomorphism with the real number system, and hence have little or no mathematical representations. An alternative in the form of an extended theory of measurements is developed. A third dimension of measurement, the supra-extensive dimension, is introduced; and a new scale, the multiplicative scale, is associated with it. It is shown that supra-extensive magnitudes do constitute systems isomorphic with the system of real numbers and that they alone can be given mathematical representations. Physical quantities are supra-extensive magnitudes. In contrast, to date, psychological quantities are either intensive or extensive, but never of the third kind. This, it is felt, is the reason why mathematical representations have been few and without success in psychology as contrasted to the physical sciences. In particular, the Weber-Fechner relation is examined and shown to be invalid in two respects. It is concluded that the construction of multiplicative scales in psychology, or the equivalent use of dimensional analysis, alone will enable the development of fruitful mathematical theories in this area of investigation.The editors of this journal should perhaps point out that unanimous agreement with the arguments and points of view expressed in this article is not anticipated. They believe, however, that its publication may stimulate needed thinking and clarification of problems basic to psychological measurement and thus serve the purpose for which the journal was founded.  相似文献   
255.
256.
Previous research aimed at testing the situational strength hypothesis suffers from serious limitations regarding the conceptualization of strength. In order to overcome these limitations, the present study attempts to test the situational strength hypothesis based on the operationalization of strength as reinforcement contingencies. One dispositional factor of proven effect on cooperative behavior, social value orientation (SVO), was used as a predictor of behavior in four social dilemmas with varying degree of situational strength. The moderating role of incentive condition (hypothetical vs. real) on the relationship between SVO and behavior was also tested. One hundred undergraduates were presented with the four social dilemmas and the Social Value Orientation Scale. One‐half of the sample played the social dilemmas using real incentives, whereas the other half used hypothetical incentives. Results supported the situational strength hypothesis in that no behavioral variability and no effect of SVO on behavior were found in the strongest situation. However, situational strength did not moderate the effect of SVO on behavior in situations where behavior showed variability. No moderating effect was found for incentive condition either. The implications of these results for personality theory and assessment are discussed.  相似文献   
257.
Although the relationship between negative childhood experiences, peer victimization, depressive symptoms, and Non-Suicidal Self-Injury (NSSI) is widely recognized, the mechanisms involved are not fully understood, especially among adolescents. This study aims to test the mediating role of both self-criticism and depressive symptoms in the relationship between memories of negative or positive experiences, current peer victimization, and NSSI. The sample consists 854 Portuguese adolescents, 451 female and 403 male, with ages between 12 and 18 years (M = 14.89; SD = 1.79), from middle and secondary schools. Participants answered self-report measures. Results from path analysis showed that memories of negative experiences, the absence of positive memories with family in childhood and peer victimization indirectly impact on NSSI through self-criticism and depressive symptoms. In addition, these stressful experiences led to depressive symptoms through self-criticism. Lastly, the most severe form of self-criticism indirectly impacts on NSSI through depressive symptoms, even though it also has a strong direct effect. It suggests that negative experiences with parents and peer victimization, as well as the absence of positive memories with family, have a negative impact on NSSI when these experiences are linked with a sense of self-hatred and depressive symptoms.  相似文献   
258.
The present study examines whether early experiences of threat, subordination and devaluation with family influence adolescents’ negative emotional states and subsequently deliberate self-harm; and if this effect is conditioned by daily peer hassles. The sample consisted of 441 adolescents (57.6 % female) with ages between 13 and 18 years old from middle and high schools. Participants completed self-report instruments measuring early memories of threat, subordination and devaluation, daily peer hassles, negative affect and deliberate self-harm behaviors. Results from conditional process analysis showed that adolescents who feel devalued and experience threat and submission within family tend to endorse high levels of negative affect, which in turn accounts for increased endorsements on deliberate self-harm. Moreover, the impact of negative affect on deliberate self-harm is amplified by the presence of moderate and high levels of daily peer hassles. This study suggests the relevance of assessing and intervening on type of emotional memories (i.e., threat, subordination and devaluation), daily disruptions with peers and negative emotional states with adolescents who self-injure. These findings may be useful in the development of preventive and intervention programs for reducing deliberate self-harm in adolescence.  相似文献   
259.
Early math skills are determined by higher cognitive processes that are particularly important for acquiring and developing skills during a child's early education. Such processes could be a critical target for identifying students at risk for math learning difficulties. Few studies have considered the use of a structural equation method to rationalize these relations. Participating in this study were 207 preschool students ages 59 to 72 months, 108 boys and 99 girls. Performance with respect to early math skills, early literacy, general intelligence, working memory, and short‐term memory was assessed. A structural equation model explaining 64.3% of the variance in early math skills was applied. Early literacy exhibited the highest statistical significance (β = 0.443, < 0.05), followed by intelligence (β = 0.286, < 0.05), working memory (β = 0.220, < 0.05), and short‐term memory (β = 0.213, < 0.05). Correlations between the independent variables were also significant (< 0.05). According to the results, cognitive variables should be included in remedial intervention programs.  相似文献   
260.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号