全文获取类型
收费全文 | 32172篇 |
免费 | 568篇 |
国内免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
32743篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 370篇 |
2019年 | 369篇 |
2018年 | 458篇 |
2017年 | 504篇 |
2016年 | 595篇 |
2015年 | 442篇 |
2014年 | 473篇 |
2013年 | 2459篇 |
2012年 | 898篇 |
2011年 | 962篇 |
2010年 | 609篇 |
2009年 | 640篇 |
2008年 | 838篇 |
2007年 | 820篇 |
2006年 | 694篇 |
2005年 | 691篇 |
2004年 | 665篇 |
2003年 | 673篇 |
2002年 | 668篇 |
2001年 | 801篇 |
2000年 | 777篇 |
1999年 | 567篇 |
1998年 | 349篇 |
1997年 | 314篇 |
1992年 | 539篇 |
1991年 | 508篇 |
1990年 | 511篇 |
1989年 | 488篇 |
1988年 | 440篇 |
1987年 | 460篇 |
1986年 | 486篇 |
1985年 | 524篇 |
1984年 | 443篇 |
1983年 | 424篇 |
1982年 | 306篇 |
1981年 | 353篇 |
1979年 | 476篇 |
1978年 | 358篇 |
1977年 | 309篇 |
1976年 | 299篇 |
1975年 | 419篇 |
1974年 | 472篇 |
1973年 | 501篇 |
1972年 | 422篇 |
1971年 | 410篇 |
1970年 | 384篇 |
1969年 | 405篇 |
1968年 | 505篇 |
1967年 | 482篇 |
1966年 | 457篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
991.
Factors associated with outcome of cognitive-behavioural treatment of chronic post-traumatic stress disorder 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
The study examined factors that were associated with outcome in the treatment of PTSD. A trial of cognitive therapy compared to imaginal exposure of chronic PTSD showed that although clinical improvements were obtained after treatment and at 6 month follow-up one type of treatment was not significantly superior to the other. Characteristics of the patient, the trauma and treatment and of pretreatment clinical measures were investigated as predictors of PTSD outcome. Eleven variables were significantly associated with the pre- to post-treatment change in CAPS severity scores. Of these, three (duration of therapy, gender and suicide risk) were selected into a step-wise multiple regression equation to explain 36.5% of the outcome. Similarly, nine variables were significant associated with the pretreatment to follow-up change with three variables (number of missed therapy sessions, residential status and co-morbid GAD) being selected into the equation and explaining 36.9% of the outcome. The best predictor of outcome was inconsistent attendance at therapy. 相似文献
992.
993.
The present study investigated the readiness potential (RP) preceding a brisk extension of the right middle finger during different arousal states as monitored by skin potential level (SPL). The late component of the readiness potential in the medium arousal state was significantly larger than those in the low and high arousal states. This finding indicates that the RP waveform may vary as a function of arousal states, suggesting the inverse U-shaped relationship proposed in studies of the contingent negative variation. 相似文献
994.
Wagemans J Van Gool L Lamote C Foster DH 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》2000,26(2):443-468
Participants judged the affine equivalence of 2 simultaneously presented 4-point patterns. Performance level (d') varied between 1.5 and 2.7, depending on the information available for solving the correspondence problem (insufficient in Experiment 1a, superfluous in Experiment 1b, and minimal in Experiments 1c, 2a, 2b) and on the exposure time (unlimited in Experiments 1 and 2a and 500 ms in Experiment 2b), but it did not vary much with the complexity of the affine transformation (rotation and slant in Experiment 1 and same plus tilt in Experiment 2). Performance in Experiment 3 was lower with 3-point patterns than with 4-point patterns, whereas blocking the trials according to the affine transformation parameters had little effect. Determining affine shape equivalence with minimal-information displays is based on a fast assessment of qualitatively or quasi-invariant properties such as convexity/ concavity, parallelism, and collinearity. 相似文献
995.
Understanding projectile acceleration 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Hecht H Bertamini M 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》2000,26(2):730-746
Throwing and catching balls or other objects is a generally highly practiced skill; however, conceptual as well as perceptual understanding of the mechanics that underlie this skill is surprisingly poor. In 5 experiments, we investigated conceptual and perceptual understanding of simple ballistic motion. Paper-and-pencil tests revealed that up to half of all participants mistakenly believed that a ball would continue to accelerate after it left the thrower's hand. Observers also showed a remarkable tolerance for anomalous trajectory shapes. Perceptual judgments based on graphics animations replicated these erroneous beliefs for shallow release angles. Observers' tolerance for anomalies tended to decrease with their distance from the actor. The findings are at odds with claims of the naive physics literature that liken intuitive understanding to Aristotelian or medieval physics theories. Instead, observers seem to project their intentions to the ball itself (externalization) or even feel that they have power over the ball when it is still close. 相似文献
996.
Relations of individuals' value priorities to their worries are investigated in seven samples from four cultural groups (N = 1,441). A social-cognitive analysis suggests that value priorities influence worries by increasing attention to and perception of threats to valued goals. On this basis, we generate hypotheses relating two types of worries, micro (about self and its extensions) and macro (about society and world), to 10 types of values. As predicted, giving priority to self-transcendence values (universalism and benevolence) is associated with low micro and high macro worry, whereas giving priority to self-enhancement values (power, hedonism, and--to a lesser degree--achievement) is associated with high micro and low macro worry. Meaningful associations are also found for other values. Values account for substantially more variance in macro than in micro worries. 相似文献
997.
Armeli S Carney MA Tennen H Affleck G O'Neil TP 《Journal of personality and social psychology》2000,78(5):979-994
The authors used a daily diary methodology to examine over 60 days how the within-person associations among event stress, alcohol consumption, and desire to drink varied as a function of gender, positive and negative alcohol-outcome expectancies, and avoidant coping in a sample of 88 regular drinkers. Multilevel regression analyses indicated that men who more strongly anticipated positive outcomes or a sense of carelessness from drinking drank relatively more on stressful days compared with low-stress days. Similar results were found predicting desire to drink. Men who anticipated greater impairment from drinking drank relatively less on stressful days. In general, these effects did not hold for women. Little evidence was found for the predicted effects for avoidant coping style, and some results showed that avoidant coping style buffered the exacerbating effects of careless unconcern expectancies. 相似文献
998.
Fischer MH 《Perception》2000,29(3):337-354
Normal readers were asked to mark the middle of visually presented words. They made systematic errors toward the left, indicating an overestimation of the length of the beginning of a word. The number of characters determines the size of this error. The bias extended to pseudowords, letter strings, and symbols, but not to blocks, dashes, and lines. Finally, the bias was sensitive to typographical errors but not to colour cuing. These findings suggest that special cognitive operations determine the perceived spatial extent of words. Implications for our understanding of perceptual and cognitive processes in reading are discussed. 相似文献
999.
Hundreds of research articles have addressed the relationship between birth order and intelligence. Virtually all have used cross-sectional data, which are fundamentally flawed in the assessment of within-family (including birth order) processes. Although within-family models have been based on patterns in cross-sectional data, a number of equally plausible between-family explanations also exist. Within-family (preferably intact-family) data are prerequisite for separating within- and between-family causal processes. This observation reframes an old issue in a way that can be easily addressed by studying graphical patterns. Sibling data from the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth are evaluated, and the results are compared with those from other studies using within-family data. It appears that although low-IQ parents have been making large families, large families do not make low-IQ children in modern U.S. society. The apparent relation between birth order and intelligence has been a methodological illusion. 相似文献
1000.
Minton HL 《The American psychologist》2000,55(6):613-615
The turn of the 20th century in America was a period of changing gender ideals. The younger generation of women pressed for economic independence and political rights. Men became caught up with the virility and physicality of the new standard of passionate manhood. These gender shifts were reflected in turn-of-the-century American psychology. With men dominating the discipline, the emerging scientific psychology projected the values of the new man. Several examples of this androcentric psychology are reviewed, including the views of such prominent psychologists as G. Stanley Hall, James McKeen Cattell, and William James. There were also a few women psychologists who challenged the androcentric bias by attempting to incorporate the values of the new woman, most notably Mary Whiton Calkins, Helen Thompson Woolley, and Leta Stetter Hollingworth. 相似文献