全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1872篇 |
免费 | 26篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
1900篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 24篇 |
2020年 | 33篇 |
2019年 | 39篇 |
2018年 | 40篇 |
2017年 | 43篇 |
2016年 | 60篇 |
2015年 | 34篇 |
2014年 | 56篇 |
2013年 | 200篇 |
2012年 | 69篇 |
2011年 | 80篇 |
2010年 | 39篇 |
2009年 | 50篇 |
2008年 | 69篇 |
2007年 | 61篇 |
2006年 | 57篇 |
2005年 | 44篇 |
2004年 | 33篇 |
2003年 | 57篇 |
2002年 | 42篇 |
2001年 | 40篇 |
2000年 | 45篇 |
1999年 | 29篇 |
1998年 | 30篇 |
1997年 | 34篇 |
1996年 | 19篇 |
1994年 | 19篇 |
1993年 | 18篇 |
1992年 | 27篇 |
1991年 | 20篇 |
1990年 | 21篇 |
1989年 | 18篇 |
1988年 | 22篇 |
1987年 | 18篇 |
1986年 | 17篇 |
1985年 | 17篇 |
1984年 | 23篇 |
1983年 | 19篇 |
1982年 | 24篇 |
1981年 | 19篇 |
1980年 | 16篇 |
1979年 | 20篇 |
1978年 | 18篇 |
1977年 | 18篇 |
1976年 | 18篇 |
1975年 | 22篇 |
1974年 | 22篇 |
1973年 | 21篇 |
1972年 | 16篇 |
1970年 | 14篇 |
排序方式: 共有1900条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
231.
232.
233.
Richard May Joanna Chick Samantha Manuel Rachel Jones 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》2019,52(3):667-681
The present study evaluated the emergence of second‐language intraverbals in typically developing young children following a small‐group teaching intervention. Choral responding was employed with a group of 6 primary school children (5‐6 years old) to teach first‐language tacts (e.g., “What is this in English?” [“Hospital”]) and related second‐language tacts (e.g., “What is this in Welsh?” [“Ysbyty”]). A multiple‐probe design across stimulus sets was used to evaluate subsequent emergence of untrained first‐to‐second‐language derived intraverbals (e.g., “What is hospital in Welsh?” [“Ysbyty”]) and untrained second‐to‐first‐language intraverbals (e.g.,”What is ysbyty in English?” [“Hospital”]). Data indicated that the choral responding intervention produced robust increases in derived intraverbal relations for 3 of the 6 participants. 相似文献
234.
Adam S. Grabell Theodore J. Huppert Frank A. Fishburn Yanwei Li Hannah M. Jones Aimee E. Wilett Lisa M. Bemis Susan B. Perlman 《Developmental science》2018,21(5)
Individual differences in young children's frustration responses set the stage for myriad developmental outcomes and represent an area of intense empirical interest. Emotion regulation is hypothesized to comprise the interplay of complex behaviors, such as facial expressions, and activation of concurrent underlying neural systems. At present, however, the literature has mostly examined children's observed emotion regulation behaviors and assumed underlying brain activation through separate investigations, resulting in theoretical gaps in our understanding of how children regulate emotion in vivo. Our goal was to elucidate links between young children's emotion regulation‐related neural activation, facial muscular movements, and parent‐rated temperamental emotion regulation. Sixty‐five children (age 3–7) completed a frustration‐inducing computer task while lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC) activation and concurrent facial expressions were recorded. Negative facial expressions with eye constriction were inversely associated with both parent‐rated temperamental emotion regulation and concurrent LPFC activation. Moreover, we found evidence that positive expressions with eye constriction during frustration may be associated with stronger LPFC activation. Results suggest a correspondence between facial expressions and LPFC activation that may explicate how children regulate emotion in real time. 相似文献
235.
Edward A. Jones 《The International journal of psycho-analysis》2017,98(4):1025-1046
This article reexamines the interpretation of the significance of the Aliquis slip and related materials in terms of a family crisis in 1900 involving Freud, Anna Freud Bernays and their youngest sister Paula. It contends that Ernest Jones and Anna Freud later hid this story from history, largely for family reasons. It supports this contention by drawing on Ernest Jones and Anna Freud's archived letters, Anna Freud Bernays’ memoirs, the complete Freud‐Fliess letters and other historical sources and compares them to The Origins of Psychoanalysis and to Ernest Jones’ biography of Freud. It shows how events during the crisis manifested themselves in Freud's On Dreams, the Aliquis slip, and the banking error story. It rejects Peter Swales’ hypotheses about Freud and Minna Bernays being part of the Aliquis slip, but accepts Didier Anzieu's idea that Minna Bernays was the subject of the banking error story. It shows that the motive for the historical repression of the story began with Freud, but was over determined and included guilt and pain that developed when Paula and other sisters got caught up in the Nazi takeover of Austria. 相似文献
236.
237.
Jamie Kwan Margaret Jones Lisa Hull Simon Wessely Nicola Fear Deirdre MacManus 《Aggressive behavior》2017,43(3):273-280
238.
Across two studies, we investigated the influence of narcissism and self-esteem along with gender on phenomenological ratings across the four subscales of the Autobiographical Memory Questionnaire (AMQ; impact, recollection, rehearsal, and belief). Memory cues varied in valence (positive vs. negative) and agency (agentic vs. communal). In Study 2, we used different memory cues reflecting these four Valence by Agency conditions and additionally investigated retrieval times for the autobiographical memories (AMs). Results were consistent with the agency model of narcissism [Campbell, W. K., Brunell, A. B., & Finkel, E. J. (2006). Narcissism, interpersonal self-regulation, and romantic relationships: An agency model approach. In E. J. Finkel & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Self and relationships: Connecting intrapersonal and interpersonal processes. New York, NY: Guilford], which characterises narcissists as being more concerned with agentic (self-focused) rather than communal (other-focused) positive self-relevant information. Narcissism predicted greater phenomenology across the four subscales for the positive-agentic memories (Study 1: clever; Study 2: attractive, talented) as well as faster memory retrieval times. Narcissism also predicted greater recollection and faster retrieval times for the negative-communal AMs (Study 1: rude; Study 2: annoying, dishonest). In contrast, self-esteem predicted greater phenomenology and faster retrieval times for the positive-communal AMs (Study 1: cooperative; Study 2: romantic, sympathetic). In both studies, results of LIWC analyses further differentiated between narcissism and self-esteem in the content (word usage) of the AMs. 相似文献
239.
Norma L. Day-Vines;Jeremy Zhang;Fallon Cluxton-Keller;Dantavious Hicks;Connie Jones;Rufus Tony Spann;Dipika Daga;Courtney Agorsor; 《Journal of multicultural counseling and development》2024,52(3):173-188
The current study evaluated the validity of the broaching attitudes and behavior survey-clients (BABS-C), a self-report measure that examined minoritized clients’ perceptions of their counselors’ ability to discuss issues of race, ethnicity, and culture during treatment. A series of confirmatory factor analyses suggested that the BABS-C framework and its subconstructs achieved excellent model fit and stable factor structures. The measurement invariance was examined with seven covariates using multiple indicators and multiple cause methods. Implications for counseling research and practice are discussed. 相似文献
240.
Debra A. Russo Jan Stochl Joanne Hodgekins Maria Iglesias-González Penelope Chipps Michelle Painter Peter B. Jones Jesus Perez 《British journal of psychology (London, England : 1953)》2018,109(1):45-62
Evidence suggests that attachment styles may influence subclinical psychosis phenotypes (schizotypy) and affective disorders and may play a part in the association between psychosis and childhood adversity. However, the role of attachment in the initial stages of psychosis remains poorly understood. Our main aim was to describe and compare attachment styles in 60 individuals at ultra high risk for psychosis (UHR) and a matched sample of 60 healthy volunteers (HV). The HV had lower anxious and avoidant attachment scores than the UHR individuals (p < .001). Sixty-nine percentage of the UHR group had more than one DSM-IV diagnosis, mainly affective and anxiety disorders. The UHR group experienced more trauma (p < .001) and more mood and anxiety symptoms (p < .001). Interestingly, in our UHR group, only schizotypy paranoia was correlated with insecure attachment. In the HV group, depression, anxiety, schizotypy paranoia, and social anxiety were correlated with insecure attachment. This difference and some discrepancies with previous studies involving UHR suggest that individuals at UHR may compose a heterogeneous group; some experience significant mood and/or anxiety symptoms that may not be explained by specific attachment styles. Nonetheless, measuring attachment in UHR individuals could help maximize therapeutic relationships to enhance recovery. 相似文献