全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2351篇 |
免费 | 64篇 |
专业分类
2415篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 10篇 |
2023年 | 10篇 |
2022年 | 19篇 |
2021年 | 31篇 |
2020年 | 58篇 |
2019年 | 76篇 |
2018年 | 99篇 |
2017年 | 104篇 |
2016年 | 93篇 |
2015年 | 69篇 |
2014年 | 101篇 |
2013年 | 315篇 |
2012年 | 122篇 |
2011年 | 149篇 |
2010年 | 88篇 |
2009年 | 83篇 |
2008年 | 108篇 |
2007年 | 105篇 |
2006年 | 80篇 |
2005年 | 82篇 |
2004年 | 97篇 |
2003年 | 73篇 |
2002年 | 67篇 |
2001年 | 28篇 |
2000年 | 24篇 |
1999年 | 28篇 |
1998年 | 17篇 |
1997年 | 18篇 |
1996年 | 31篇 |
1995年 | 22篇 |
1994年 | 23篇 |
1993年 | 15篇 |
1992年 | 12篇 |
1991年 | 10篇 |
1990年 | 7篇 |
1989年 | 10篇 |
1988年 | 7篇 |
1987年 | 8篇 |
1986年 | 5篇 |
1985年 | 9篇 |
1984年 | 15篇 |
1983年 | 5篇 |
1982年 | 9篇 |
1981年 | 7篇 |
1980年 | 13篇 |
1979年 | 9篇 |
1978年 | 8篇 |
1977年 | 5篇 |
1975年 | 9篇 |
1973年 | 5篇 |
排序方式: 共有2415条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
251.
252.
Paulina Flasch Dodie Limberg‐Ohrt Jesse Fox Jonathan H. Ohrt Elizabeth Crunk Edward Robinson 《Counseling and values》2019,64(2):168-185
The purpose of this phenomenological investigation was to explore experiences of altruism in the therapeutic relationship to better understand how counselors‐in‐training (CITs) and their clients mutually perceived the construct in sessions. Findings from interviews with clients (n = 14) included emotional–relational characteristics and cognitive–behavioral aspects of altruism by their counselors. Findings from interviews with CITs (n = 10) included explorations of the role of altruism in counseling, overall experiences of altruism in counseling sessions, and experiences of altruism with specific clients. Implications for practice and research are discussed. 相似文献
253.
254.
Tiffany M. Bordonada Katherine A. Feather Jonathan H. Ohrt Ashley F. Waddington 《Adultspan: Theory Research & Practice》2018,17(1):14-26
The role of primary caregiver is unique. Understanding the role of the primary caregiver will better inform those in the counseling profession. The purpose of this qualitative research study was to gain a clearer perspective of the lived experiences of adults who identify as primary caregivers. The authors conducted semistructured interviews with 6 Caucasian female participants and analyzed the data using interpretative phenomenological analysis. The findings include personal perceptions and caregiver experiences pertaining to the caregiver role, caregiver mentality, professional identity, social and emotional responses, and self‐care among caregivers throughout the caregiving process. Implications for counselors and future research are presented. 相似文献
255.
Jonathan L. Shaheen 《Inquiry (Oslo, Norway)》2017,60(8):839-866
This paper presents evidence that ‘because’ is importantly ambiguous between two closely related senses covering what are usually called causal explanations, on the one hand, and grounding or metaphysical explanations, on the other hand. To this end, it introduces the lexical categories of monosemy, polysemy and homonymy; describes a test for polysemy; and discusses the results of the test when applied to ‘because’. It also shows how to understand so-called hybrid explanations in light of the semantic facts established by the analysis. 相似文献
256.
Jonathan Livingstone-Banks 《Inquiry (Oslo, Norway)》2017,60(8):816-838
Kit Fine’s arguments in Essence and Modality are widely accepted as being a decisive blow against modal essentialism. A selection of replies exist that have done little to counter the general view that modally construed essence is out of touch with what we really mean when we make essentialist claims. I argue that Fine’s arguments fail to strike a decisive blow, and I suggest a new interpretation of the debate that shows why Fine’s arguments fall short of achieving their goal. 相似文献
257.
Jonathan W. Pinkston Benjamin M. Libman 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》2017,108(1):73-96
Historically, effort has been viewed as aversive. Most supporting evidence comes from studies demonstrating increased force/effort requirements reduce operant responding. Changes in force/effort requirements, however, are often accompanied by changes in response definition when mechanical devices are used to define the response. As a consequence, responses measured at one point in a study may go unmeasured at other points. In an alternative approach, we used a continuous measurement strategy that provided a means to fix the threshold force defining the response class and simultaneously allowed independent manipulation of the force criteria required to produce reinforcement. Rats pressed a force transducer according to a fixed‐ratio 5 schedule of food delivery. The criterion force was systematically increased and decreased; the threshold for response detection was constant. When response rates included only criterion responses, overall rate decreased when force requirements increased. By contrast, when all responses, both those meeting force criteria and those that did not (above the threshold but below the criteria for reinforcement) were included in the rate calculation, increases in force increased response rate. Increases in force criteria also increased the maximum force (g) and time‐integral of force (g‐s) of operant behavior. Control conditions showed increases in responding could be explained by the emergence of subcriterion responses, irrespective of force. We conclude that prior results showing effort decreases response rates are due to an artifact arising from inadvertent changes in response definitions. Increases in effort may better be understood as changes in the response:reinforcer payoff owing to the emergence of a subcriterion response class. 相似文献
258.
Jonathan Buriticá Cristiano V. dos Santos 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》2017,108(2):151-170
The concept of reinforcement value summarizes the effect of different variables, such as reinforcement delay, reinforcement magnitude, and deprivation level, on behavior. In the present set of experiments, we evaluated the effect of reinforcement devaluation on performance under FI schedules. The literature on timing and reinforcement value suggests that devaluation generates longer expected times to reinforcement than the same intervals trained under control conditions. We devalued reinforcement with delay in Experiments 1A, 1B, and 2, and diminished deprivation in Experiments 3A and 3B. Devaluation reduced response rates, increased the number of one‐response intervals, and lengthened postreinforcement pauses, but had inconsistent effects on other timing measures such as quarter life and breakpoint. The results of delayed reinforcement and diminished deprivation manipulations are well summarized as reinforcement devaluation effects. These results suggest that devaluation may reduce stimulus control. In addition, we argue that the process by which delayed reinforcement affects behavior might also explain some effects observed in other devaluation procedures through the concept of reinforcement value. 相似文献
259.
Black Mountain College is remembered as an artistic utopian alternative to institutional learning. Its faculty and students included some of the most important creative thinkers of the 20th century. Its foundation was built on the philosophy of “learning by doing.” But what made Black Mountain such a dynamic educational environment? Today, the financial burden of higher education places a lasting strain on students that inhibits creative growth. Does the educational structure of the college system impede our learning? Black Mountain was a legendary educational experiment, which offers a model for contemporary art education. 相似文献
260.