全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2358篇 |
免费 | 64篇 |
专业分类
2422篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 10篇 |
2023年 | 10篇 |
2022年 | 19篇 |
2021年 | 31篇 |
2020年 | 58篇 |
2019年 | 76篇 |
2018年 | 99篇 |
2017年 | 104篇 |
2016年 | 94篇 |
2015年 | 69篇 |
2014年 | 101篇 |
2013年 | 316篇 |
2012年 | 123篇 |
2011年 | 149篇 |
2010年 | 88篇 |
2009年 | 83篇 |
2008年 | 108篇 |
2007年 | 105篇 |
2006年 | 80篇 |
2005年 | 82篇 |
2004年 | 97篇 |
2003年 | 73篇 |
2002年 | 67篇 |
2001年 | 28篇 |
2000年 | 25篇 |
1999年 | 28篇 |
1998年 | 17篇 |
1997年 | 18篇 |
1996年 | 31篇 |
1995年 | 22篇 |
1994年 | 23篇 |
1993年 | 15篇 |
1992年 | 12篇 |
1991年 | 10篇 |
1990年 | 7篇 |
1989年 | 11篇 |
1988年 | 7篇 |
1987年 | 9篇 |
1986年 | 5篇 |
1985年 | 9篇 |
1984年 | 15篇 |
1983年 | 5篇 |
1982年 | 9篇 |
1981年 | 8篇 |
1980年 | 13篇 |
1979年 | 9篇 |
1978年 | 8篇 |
1977年 | 5篇 |
1975年 | 9篇 |
1973年 | 5篇 |
排序方式: 共有2422条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
161.
The effect of proprioceptive adaptation to visual displacement is to produce a violation of the normal constraints on the relative position of body parts. In order to investigate the effect of this violation of constraints on the adaptation, the relative position of body parts for a group of dancers and a group of nondancers was determined after 5 and 15 min of adaptation. Empirical and theoretical support were provided for the propositions that (1) there is a tendency to resist the violation of constraints, and (2) the nature of the adaptation process is such as to minimize the amount of violation of constraints. 相似文献
162.
Dr. Edward W. Schultz John A. Salvia Jonathan Feinn 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1974,2(1):17-24
Third- and fourth grade children attending rural midwestem elementary schools were assessed for the presence of fiftyfive behavior symptoms. In general, prevalence rates were higher for boys and reflected behavioral patterns commonly associated with conduct problems and immaturity. More neurotic forms of behavior were reported for girls. Additionally, an increase in symptoms per sex across grade level was found with fourth grade girls showing an increase common to the conduct disorder cluster. 相似文献
163.
Sheila E. Blumstein William E. Cooper Harold Goodglass Sheila Statlender Jonathan Gottlieb 《Brain and language》1980,9(2):153-170
An experimental study was conducted to examine phonetic and phonemic deficits in the speech production of aphasics. Subjects included four Broca's aphasics, four Conduction aphasics, five Wernicke's aphasics, one nonaphasic dysarthric patient, and four normal controls. The subjects read a list of words containing word-initial stop consonants which were subsequently measured acoustically for voice-onset time. The results showed that Broca's aphasics exhibit a more severe production disorder than Conduction aphasics who in turn exhibit a more severe disorder than Wernicke's aphasics, in accord with clinical observations. In addition, although Broca's aphasics produced both phonetic and phonemic errors, the results showed that they have a pervasive phonetic disorder which affects their correct target productions as well as the total number of phonetic errors produced. This deficit however seems to be a speech deficit rather than a low-level motor control problem. In contrast, the Wernicke's aphasics show a deficit characterized by isolated phonemic mistargeting errors. Finally, the pattern of productions for the Conduction aphasics indicates that some patients show a predominantly phonetic disorder similar to the Broca's aphasics and others show predominantly a phonemic disorder similar to the Wernicke's aphasics. 相似文献
164.
Elizabeth F. Loftus Jonathan W. Schooler Stanley M. Boone Donald Kline 《Applied cognitive psychology》1987,1(1):3-13
In three experiments, 469 subjects watched a short videotape of a bank robbery and later estimated the duration of the tape. Subjects invariably overestimated the durations. Accuracy of time estimation was unrelated to amount of free recall (Experiment 1) or accuracy of memory (Experiment 2). Females overestimated to a greater degree than males (Experiments 2 and 3). A more stressful version of the event produced greater overestimates than a less stressful version (Experiment 3). The relationship between induced arousal and time estimation appears to be different for men and women. 相似文献
165.
Jonathan Knowles 《The Philosophical quarterly》1999,49(195):164-181
I focus on Fodor's model of the relationship between special sciences and basic physics, and on a criticism of this model, that it implies that the causal stability of, e.g., the mental in its production of behaviour is nothing short of a miraculous coincidence. David Papineau and Graham Macdonaldendorse this criticism. But it is far less clear than they assume that Fodor's picture indeed involves coincidences, which in any case their injection of a teleological supplement cannot explain. Papineau's and Macdonald's problem is subtly different from a similar one presented by Adrian Cussins. This is no more effective against Fodor's picture, but the kind of account of the relation between the physical and the psychological which could constitute a solution to Cussins' problem is one which, for independent reasons, a physicalist of Fodor's stripe ought to provide. 相似文献
166.
Four perceptual identification experiments examined the influence of spatial cues on the recognition of words presented in central vision (with fixation on either the first or last letter of the target word) and in peripheral vision (displaced left or right of a central fixation point). Stimulus location had a strong effect on word identification accuracy in both central and peripheral vision, showing a strong right visual field superiority that did not depend on eccentricity. Valid spatial cues improved word identification for peripherally presented targets but were largely ineffective for centrally presented targets. Effects of spatial cuing interacted with visual field effects in Experiment 1, with valid cues reducing the right visual field superiority for peripherally located targets, but this interaction was shown to depend on the type of neutral cue. These results provide further support for the role of attentional factors in visual field asymmetries obtained with targets in peripheral vision but not with centrally presented targets. 相似文献
167.
Hadjichristidis C Handley SJ Sloman SA Evans JS Over DE Stevenson RJ 《Memory & cognition》2007,35(8):2052-2059
The ability to entertain possibilities and draw inferences about them is essential to human intelligence. We examine the hypothesis that conditional if-then statements trigger a mental simulation process in which people suppose the antecedent (if statement) to be true and evaluate the consequent (then statement) in that context. On the assumption that supposing an event to be true increases belief that the event has occurred or will occur, this hypothesis is consistent with the claim that evaluating a conditional will heighten belief in its antecedent more than in its consequent. Two experiments, employing conditionals of the form If animal A has property X, then animal B will have property X, in which X was a property that people could not readily relate to the animals, supported this claim. The effect was stronger following the evaluation of conditionals with dissimilar animal categories. 相似文献
168.
Elizabeth F. Loftus Karen Donders Hunter G. Hoffman Jonathan W. Schooler 《Memory & cognition》1989,17(5):607-616
In two experiments involving a total of 542 subjects, a series of slides depicting a burglary was shown. After the initial event, subjects were exposed to one or more narratives about the event that contained some misinformation or neutral information about four critical details. Finally, subjects were tested on their memories of what they saw, and their reaction times and confidence levels were measured. When subjects took a standard test in which the misinformation item was a possible response option, they responded very quickly and confidently when making this incorrect choice. Misled subjects responded as quickly and confidently to these "unreal" memories as they did to their genuine memories. It does not seem, then, that the misinformation effect arises from a large proportion of subjects who must resolve a conflict between two memories when they are tested, a conflict that would be expected to take time. When subjects took a modified test in which the misinformation item was not a possible response, misled subjects were as accurate as were controls, but they responded more slowly, regardless of whether they ultimately chose the right or wrong option. These findings indicate that misinformation does introduce some form of interference not detected by a simple test of accuracy. 相似文献
169.
Perceiving geographical slant 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Dennis R. Proffitt Mukul Bhalla Rich Gossweiler Jonathan Midgett 《Psychonomic bulletin & review》1995,2(4):409-428
People judged the inclination of hills viewed either out-of-doors or in a computer-simulated virtual environment. Angle judgments were obtained by having people (1) provide verbal estimates, (2) adjust a representation of the hill’s cross-section, and (3) adjust a tilt board with their unseen hand. Geographical slant was greatly overestimated according to the first two measures, but not the third. Apparent slant judgments conformed to ratio scales, thereby enhancing sensitivity to the small inclines that must actually be traversed in everyday experience. It is proposed that the perceived exaggeration of geographical slant preserves the relationship between distal inclination and people’s behavioral potential. Hills are harder to traverse as people become tired; hence, apparent slant increased with fatigue. Visually guided actions must be accommodated to the actual distal properties of the environment; consequently, the tilt board adjustments did not reflect apparent slant overestimations, nor were they influenced by fatigue. Consistent with the fact that steep hills are more difficult to descend than to ascend, these hills appeared steeper when viewed from the top. 相似文献
170.
Grainger J Diependaele K Spinelli E Ferrand L Farioli F 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2003,29(6):1256-1269
Lexical decision latencies to word targets presented either visually or auditorily were faster when directly preceded by a briefly presented (53-ms) pattern-masked visual prime that was the same word as the target (repetition primes), compared with different word primes. Primes that were pseudohomophones of target words did not significantly influence target processing compared with unrelated primes (Experiments 1-2) but did produce robust priming effects with slightly longer prime exposures (67 ms) in Experiment 3. Like repetition priming, these pseudohomophone priming effects did not interact with target modality. Experiments 4 and 5 replicated this general pattern of effects while introducing a different measure of prime visibility and an orthographic priming condition. Results are interpreted within the framework of a bimodal interactive activation model. 相似文献