全文获取类型
收费全文 | 44693篇 |
免费 | 823篇 |
国内免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
45519篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 433篇 |
2019年 | 585篇 |
2018年 | 777篇 |
2017年 | 852篇 |
2016年 | 819篇 |
2015年 | 626篇 |
2014年 | 729篇 |
2013年 | 3793篇 |
2012年 | 1277篇 |
2011年 | 1424篇 |
2010年 | 943篇 |
2009年 | 936篇 |
2008年 | 1325篇 |
2007年 | 1218篇 |
2006年 | 1158篇 |
2005年 | 1036篇 |
2004年 | 1004篇 |
2003年 | 1016篇 |
2002年 | 989篇 |
2001年 | 1401篇 |
2000年 | 1335篇 |
1999年 | 1024篇 |
1998年 | 571篇 |
1997年 | 494篇 |
1996年 | 441篇 |
1995年 | 460篇 |
1994年 | 455篇 |
1993年 | 457篇 |
1992年 | 875篇 |
1991年 | 821篇 |
1990年 | 800篇 |
1989年 | 730篇 |
1988年 | 696篇 |
1987年 | 702篇 |
1986年 | 656篇 |
1985年 | 740篇 |
1984年 | 649篇 |
1983年 | 549篇 |
1982年 | 477篇 |
1981年 | 462篇 |
1979年 | 587篇 |
1978年 | 516篇 |
1977年 | 481篇 |
1976年 | 461篇 |
1975年 | 549篇 |
1974年 | 569篇 |
1973年 | 577篇 |
1972年 | 454篇 |
1968年 | 447篇 |
1966年 | 443篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
81.
TOWARD A FEMINIST UNDERSTANDING OF WOMEN AND POWER 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Both psychologists and feminists believe power is an important and ubiquitous concept, yet its definition and scope eludes both groups. In this introduction to a special issue on women and power, we suggest three points to help organize and interpret research in the area. First, definitions of power should center around the distinction between "power-over," the domination and control of one person or group over another, and "power-to" or personal empowerment. Second, power can be analyzed at different levels—societal, organizational, interpersonal, and individual—and, importantly, these levels interact. Third, power differences frequently underlie what appear to be gender differences in behavior; as society is currently configured, power and gender are never independent. Although the articles in this special issue often ask more questions than they answer, the present volume adds a feminist perspective to the psychological study of power. 相似文献
82.
We demonstrate the power of a situated identity perspective for understanding the different attributional patterns of women and men in an academic setting and their differential experiences in the workplace. Two explanations for the gender difference found in attributions of success are considered. This difference may be due either to different identities being attributed to men and women employing the same explanations for success and failure, or to the inconsistency between actions that confirm a professional (academic) identity and women's gender identity. The results of this survey of senior social science faculty men support the latter explanation but not the former. An editorial acceptance was seen as more professional but less feminine than a rejection. In the eyes of senior colleagues, the modest account of success, typical of women academics, enhances femininity but detracts from professionalism. The self-serving account typical of men makes the offerer appear less feminine but more professional. Thus in their situated identity claims, successful academic women, but not men, must choose between their professional and gender identities. Despite the movement of women into university social science positions, the role of academic has a masculine face.Order of authors was determined by a coin toss. 相似文献
83.
84.
85.
The purpose of this pilot study was to assess the effectiveness of a brief test user orientation program on increasing service station managers' familiarity with a few key employment testing issues, including their familiarity with theModel Guidelines for Preemployment Integrity Testing Programs. The obtained results reveal that even a brief orientation program was able to significantly increase the managers' stated degree of familiarity with testing issues. 相似文献
86.
Michael S. Wogalter D. Bradley Marwitz David C. Leonard 《Applied cognitive psychology》1992,6(5):443-453
The present research examined whether line-ups based on target (‘suspect’) face similarity are biased or suggestive. Four experiments are described in which subjects constructed photographic line-ups by selecting foils similar in appearance to a target. Later, another group of subjects who had not seen the faces before (mock witnesses) were asked to pick out the targets from the line-ups. All four experiments showed that mock witnesses selected the target significantly more often than expected by chance, thereby demonstrating suggestiveness. Three alternative line-up construction methods were also evaluated. In these methods, foil selection was based not only on target similarity but also on similarity with one or more of the other line-up faces. Results showed that alternative line-up targets were not selected significantly more often than chance, suggesting that bias was reduced. An overall analysis showed that the alternative line-ups were significantly less suggestive than target-based line-ups. The results indicate that foil selection procedures that incorporate foil-to-foil similarity produce fairer line-ups than those exclusively based on target similarity. 相似文献
87.
John Radford 《Applied cognitive psychology》1992,6(5):455-455
88.
89.
The present study aimed to determine whether there was a systematic relationship between Rogers'(1957) core conditions of empathy, acceptance and genuineness, and counsellors' use of specific behaviours or skills which have been isolated for training. Video recordings of 12 counsellors' performance in 'naturalistic' counselling interactions were analysed. Counsellor, client and judges' ratings of the core conditions were obtained. Skill assessment included molar ratings of eight skill areas and molecular measures of 29 behaviours. The results indicated that there were few significant relationships. It is concluded that there is a discrepancy between specification of the skills or behaviours which are thought to communicate the core conditions and those which have been empirically demonstrated to do so. Implications for counsellor training and proposals for future research are discussed. 相似文献
90.