全文获取类型
收费全文 | 13506篇 |
免费 | 233篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
13740篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 79篇 |
2020年 | 142篇 |
2019年 | 178篇 |
2018年 | 254篇 |
2017年 | 273篇 |
2016年 | 283篇 |
2015年 | 214篇 |
2014年 | 278篇 |
2013年 | 1465篇 |
2012年 | 477篇 |
2011年 | 515篇 |
2010年 | 329篇 |
2009年 | 330篇 |
2008年 | 498篇 |
2007年 | 460篇 |
2006年 | 444篇 |
2005年 | 441篇 |
2004年 | 447篇 |
2003年 | 428篇 |
2002年 | 458篇 |
2001年 | 224篇 |
2000年 | 194篇 |
1999年 | 205篇 |
1998年 | 219篇 |
1997年 | 214篇 |
1996年 | 174篇 |
1995年 | 178篇 |
1994年 | 166篇 |
1993年 | 184篇 |
1992年 | 172篇 |
1991年 | 147篇 |
1990年 | 150篇 |
1989年 | 131篇 |
1988年 | 128篇 |
1987年 | 135篇 |
1986年 | 120篇 |
1985年 | 152篇 |
1984年 | 192篇 |
1983年 | 195篇 |
1982年 | 195篇 |
1981年 | 178篇 |
1980年 | 144篇 |
1979年 | 135篇 |
1978年 | 180篇 |
1977年 | 162篇 |
1976年 | 166篇 |
1975年 | 144篇 |
1974年 | 156篇 |
1973年 | 111篇 |
1972年 | 78篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
291.
John D. Thomas 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1976,9(2):209-210
Being able to assess one's own performance would seem to be prerequisite to most forms of self-management. The present study investigated the extent to which children in a typical second-grade classroom could accurately assess their own academic on-task behavior. In essence, this investigation replicated an experiment by Glynn and Thomas (Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1974, 7 , 299–306) and extended those findings, by studying self-assessment over a longer span of time. The 31 children in the class ranged in age from 7 yr five months to 8 yr six months. Although only eight “problem” children were observed and studied, the self-control treatment procedures were applied to all children in the class. Behavior was observed daily during mathematics lessons for over eight school weeks. The study utilized a five-phase ABABC design, where the A phases were baseline periods, B phases were self-control periods, and C phase was a postcheck. In the self-control phases, children were asked by the teacher to put a check on their own individual performance card if they were on-task when a signal sounded. If they were not on-task, they were instructed not to touch their card. Typically, 15 to 20 signals occurred per 40-min session. At the end of the lesson, children were allowed to choose games to play according to how many checks they had earned. During all experimental phases, observers measured children's on-task behavior. Other observers measured the children's accuracy of self-assessment in the self-control phases. The results showed noticeable increases in the daily mean on-task behavior scores in the self-control phases following baseline phases. Also, a high level of on-task behavior was maintained in the postcheck phase. The variances of on-task behavior scores during the self-control phases were noticeably smaller than those in the baseline phases. Generally, individual data reflected group results, with some minor differences. Levels of accuracy of self-assessment varied from very accurate (95% of the time) to relatively inaccurate (56% of the time). The overall level of accuracy for all children was 78%. No consistent individual pattern appeared across phases. Some children got better as the study proceeded and some got worse. Most subjects tended to give themselves too much reinforcement, rather than too little. Peer social surveillance appeared to be the major factor that influenced individuals' evaluation of their own behavior, even though the teacher did make some control remarks in regard to how the children marked their cards. The results also demonstrated that self-control techniques can increase on-task behavior in a classroom with no history of external reinforcement, and that these techniques can have a lasting effect for more than two months of a school year. 相似文献
292.
293.
Four hamsters received brief electric shocks contingent upon running in a wheel. The fixed interval between shocks was varied systematically from 0 to 60 sec. Time spent running was directly related to the length of the fixed interval, while running speed remained roughly constant. Running depended on the fixed interval between shocks in the same way as bar-pressing maintained by food reinforcement, despite the fact that running was not established with nor maintained by explicit reinforcement. 相似文献
294.
295.
The effect of incongruent color words on speed of classifying ink colors was measured in visual scanning tasks and in card sorting tasks. In both cases, little or no interference effects were noted when the classification allowed focusing on a single ink color or a set of highly similar colors (adjacent hues). Substantial interference occurred when the task required grouping of three dissimilar colors (nonadjacent hues). These findings suggest that the relative efficiency of name and visual codes in making perceptual classifications is largely dependent upon the memory requirements imposed by the task. 相似文献
296.
The effects of stress on trait and state anxiety of emotionally disturbed, normal, and delinquent children were assessed using the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children (STAIC). This scale measures both transitory feelings of apprehension and tension that vary over time as a function of situational stress (A-State) and individual differences in the disposition to experience such anxiety states (A-Trait). Results indicated that A-State increased as a function of stress, whereas A-Trait was relatively stable and impervious to the temporary effects of stress. Use of the STAIC with normal and deviant populations was supported. 相似文献
297.
298.
David E. Anderson John E. Yingling Joseph V. Brady 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》1976,11(3):150-161
Laboratory dogs were trained to press a response panel to postpone shocks during daily one-hour avoidance conditioning periods. Each dog was also confined in the experimental environment for 5 hours prior to the avoidance periods. Blood pressure and heart rate were monitored continuously during these experiments from chronically indwelling arterial catheters. Extended training resulted in the emergence of a cardiovascular response pattern during the pre-avoidance interval characterized by gradual increases in blood pressure together with decreases in heart rate. Elevations in both blood pressure and heart rate were sustained during the avoidance periods. During sessions in which alpha adrenergic activity was suppressed by phenoxybenzamine, absolute levels of blond pressure were found to be lower than during control (non-drug) sessions, but a progressive rise in blood pressure continued to be observed during pre-avoidance. These results suggest that sustained cardiovascular responses during avoidance periods are associated with activation of the sympathetic nervous system, but that the gradual rise in blood pressure during pre-avoidance is due to other factors. 相似文献
299.
Paul J. Lavrakas John R. Buri Mark S. Mayzner 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1976,20(6):475-481
The purpose of the present study is to provide a perspective on the recognition of other-race faces, i.e., black faces by white subjects. Past research has indicated that individuals have greater difficulty recognizing other-race faces than they do same-race faces. On the basis of previous findings, we chose to investigate the recognition of black faces by white subjects as a function of the quantity and quality of previous experience with blacks, racial attitudes, field dependence/independence, and recognition training. Forty-two white subjects participated in pre-, post-, and delayed recognition tests. Subject data were also collected to explore individual differences in recognition performance. Using multiple regression techniques, it was found that field dependence/independence accounted for the largest proportion of variance in recognition performances; a feature-discrimination training task and past experience with blacks were also found to significantly relate to recognition performance. The applied aspects of these results are discussed. 相似文献
300.
Randolph Blake John M. Camisa Dianne N. Antoinetti 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1976,20(2):113-118
Using both the method of adjustment and forced-choice techniques, it was found that binocular depth thresholds depend on the orientation of the test targets, which in these experiments consisted of long, thin rods. Stereoacuity is greatest with vertical rods and decreases progressively as the angle of orientation approaches horizontal. The relationship between stereoacuity and orientation is governed by the orientation of the images of the targets on the retina, and is predicted fairly well by the sine of the angle of orientation, suggesting an equivalence between angular rotation and reductions in rod length. 相似文献