首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   48645篇
  免费   1925篇
  国内免费   18篇
  50588篇
  2020年   540篇
  2019年   692篇
  2018年   933篇
  2017年   1020篇
  2016年   1028篇
  2015年   728篇
  2014年   912篇
  2013年   4217篇
  2012年   1640篇
  2011年   1676篇
  2010年   1082篇
  2009年   1049篇
  2008年   1546篇
  2007年   1529篇
  2006年   1392篇
  2005年   1279篇
  2004年   1226篇
  2003年   1158篇
  2002年   1204篇
  2001年   1361篇
  2000年   1268篇
  1999年   1043篇
  1998年   570篇
  1997年   559篇
  1996年   485篇
  1995年   506篇
  1994年   490篇
  1993年   495篇
  1992年   879篇
  1991年   767篇
  1990年   829篇
  1989年   709篇
  1988年   737篇
  1987年   715篇
  1986年   704篇
  1985年   673篇
  1984年   673篇
  1983年   647篇
  1982年   513篇
  1981年   474篇
  1979年   685篇
  1978年   550篇
  1977年   481篇
  1976年   464篇
  1975年   575篇
  1974年   629篇
  1973年   627篇
  1972年   468篇
  1968年   535篇
  1967年   478篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
11.
12.
The authors tested effects of a 10-week group cognitive-behavioral stress management intervention among 100 women newly treated for Stage 0-II breast cancer. The intervention reduced prevalence of moderate depression (which remained relatively stable in the control condition) but did not affect other measures of emotional distress. The intervention also increased participants' reports that having breast cancer had made positive contributions to their lives, and it increased generalized optimism. Both remained significantly elevated at a 3-month follow-up of the intervention. Further analysis revealed that the intervention had its greatest impact on these 2 variables among women who were lowest in optimism at baseline. Discussion centers on the importance of examining positive responses to traumatic events--growth, appreciation of life, shift in priorities, and positive affect-as well as negative responses.  相似文献   
13.
14.
15.
16.
17.
18.
19.
Three experiments, each using a single group of pigeons, are reported. In Experiment 1 subjects were initially trained with two stimuli, one of which was always followed by food, the other being reinforced according to a 50% partial reinforcement schedule. Subsequently a serial procedure was adopted in which an additional stimulus, C, was consistently followed by the partially reinforced CS. A second additional stimulus, A, was followed on half of its occurrences by the continuously reinforced CS, its remaining presentations being followed by nothing. The rate of autoshaped keypecking was substantially greater during A than during C. In the remaining experiments subjects received first-order conditioning with a single stimulus that was either partially (Experiment 2) or continuously (Experiment 3) reinforced. The stimuli A and C were then again introduced for serial autoshaping. Stimulus A was occasionally paired with the CS and occasionally followed by nothing, whereas stimulus C was always followed by the CS. As in Experiment 1, the rate of responding during A was greater than during C. It is proposed that one influence on the rate of autoshaped keypecking during a CS is the accuracy with which the immediate consequences of that CS are predicted.  相似文献   
20.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号