全文获取类型
收费全文 | 35542篇 |
免费 | 636篇 |
国内免费 | 4篇 |
专业分类
36182篇 |
出版年
2019年 | 378篇 |
2018年 | 545篇 |
2017年 | 620篇 |
2016年 | 624篇 |
2015年 | 436篇 |
2014年 | 560篇 |
2013年 | 3033篇 |
2012年 | 1038篇 |
2011年 | 1115篇 |
2010年 | 707篇 |
2009年 | 687篇 |
2008年 | 1024篇 |
2007年 | 977篇 |
2006年 | 911篇 |
2005年 | 864篇 |
2004年 | 863篇 |
2003年 | 805篇 |
2002年 | 872篇 |
2001年 | 947篇 |
2000年 | 921篇 |
1999年 | 739篇 |
1998年 | 461篇 |
1997年 | 414篇 |
1996年 | 361篇 |
1993年 | 367篇 |
1992年 | 609篇 |
1991年 | 544篇 |
1990年 | 585篇 |
1989年 | 540篇 |
1988年 | 520篇 |
1987年 | 525篇 |
1986年 | 516篇 |
1985年 | 571篇 |
1984年 | 519篇 |
1983年 | 483篇 |
1982年 | 421篇 |
1981年 | 393篇 |
1979年 | 525篇 |
1978年 | 454篇 |
1977年 | 412篇 |
1976年 | 374篇 |
1975年 | 486篇 |
1974年 | 524篇 |
1973年 | 464篇 |
1972年 | 412篇 |
1971年 | 358篇 |
1969年 | 392篇 |
1968年 | 433篇 |
1967年 | 376篇 |
1966年 | 389篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
951.
Category-specific impairments of object recognition and naming are among the most intriguing disorders in neuropsychology, affecting the retrieval of knowledge about either living or nonliving things. They can give us insight into the nature of our representations of objects: Have we evolved different neural systems for recognizing different categories of object? What kinds of knowledge are important for recognizing particular objects? How does visual similarity within a category influence object recognition and representation? What is the nature of our semantic knowledge about different objects? We review the evidence on category-specific impairments, arguing that deficits even for one class of object (e.g., living things) cannot be accounted for in terms of a single information processing disorder across all patients; problems arise at contrasting loci in different patients. The same apparent pattern of impairment can be produced by damage to different loci. According to a new processing framework for object recognition and naming, the hierarchical interactive theory (HIT), we have a hierarchy of highly interactive stored representations. HIT explains the variety of patients in terms of (1) lesions at different levels of processing and (2) different forms of stored knowledge used both for particular tasks and for particular categories of object. 相似文献
952.
Examined the free-field interaction of 32 mother-child dyads who volunteered to participate in a 1-hr home observation. Observers coded mother instructions, child compliance, and child prosocial approaches plus mothers' social attention as potential reinforcers for the children's compliance and social approaches. Herrnstein's matching law was used to analyze covariations between mothers' attention and the children's 2 responses. This analysis was followed by correlational and sequential probability analyses to determine linkages between these 2 child responses and the children's willingness to obey their mothers' instructions. Results showed consistent matching between mothers' social attention and the children's production of prosocial approaches and acts of compliance. An index of the proportions of these 2 responses also covaried with the children's compliance probabilities, and the prosocial approach component was the direct covariate. These findings are discussed within an interactional synchrony framework in which children's willingness to obey their mothers is influenced by opportunities for the dyad to engage each other in specific forms of social interaction. 相似文献
953.
During bimanual movements, two relatively stable "inherent" patterns of coordination (in-phase and anti-phase) are displayed (e.g., Kelso, Am. J. Physiol. 246 (1984) R1000). Recent research has shown that new patterns of coordination can be learned. For example, following practice a 90 degrees out-of-phase pattern can emerge as an additional, relatively stable, state (e.g., Zanone & Kelso, J. Exp. Psychol.: Human Performance and Perception 18 (1992) 403). On this basis, it has been concluded that practice leads to the evolution and stabilisation of the newly learned pattern and that this process of learning changes the entire attractor layout of the dynamic system. A general feature of such research has been to observe the changes of the targeted pattern's stability characteristics during training at a single movement frequency. The present study was designed to examine how practice affects the maintenance of a coordinated pattern as the movement frequency is scaled. Eleven volunteers were asked to perform a bimanual forearm pronation-supination task. Time to transition onset was used as an index of the subjects' ability to maintain two symmetrically opposite coordinated patterns (target task - 90 degrees out-of-phase - transfer task - 270 degrees out-of-phase). Their ability to maintain the target task and the transfer task were examined again after five practice sessions each consisting of 15 trials of only the 90 degrees out-of-phase pattern. Concurrent performance feedback (a Lissajous figure) was available to the participants during each practice trial. A comparison of the time to transition onset showed that the target task was more stable after practice (p=0.025). These changes were still observed one week (p=0.05) and two months (p=0.075) after the practice period. Changes in the stability of the transfer task were not observed until two months after practice (p=0.025). Notably, following practice, transitions from the 90 degrees pattern were generally to the anti-phase (180 degrees ) pattern, whereas, transitions from the 270 degrees pattern were to the 90 degrees pattern. These results suggest that practice does improve the stability of a 90 degrees pattern, and that such improvements are transferable to the performance of the unpractised 270 degrees pattern. In addition, the anti-phase pattern remained more stable than the practised 90 degrees pattern throughout. 相似文献
954.
G Sprang 《Psychological reports》2001,89(2):331-338
This study explored the intermediate psychological effects of terrorism on adults not directly affected by the Oklahoma City bombing by examining the course of PTSD and subthreshold PTSD symptoms over time and whether treatment affected this course. The respondents were interviewed at 3-mo. intervals for 18 mo. following an initial 6-mo. survey. Analysis suggested avoidance, re-experiencing, and increased arousal symptoms in this population were limited over time, declining with or without treatment between 6 and 9 mo. Conversely, victimization symptoms remained high without mental health intervention for the first year after the disaster. The study also examined the nature and occurrence of comorbidity in groups seeking and not seeking treatment. 相似文献
955.
The representation of serial killers was examined from the analysis of 317 Web pages in the Italian language to study how the psychological profiles of serial killers are described on the Italian Internet. The correspondence analysis of the content of these Web pages shows that in Italy the serial killer is associated with words such as "monster" and "horror," which suggest and imply psychological perversion and aberrant acts. These traits are peculiar for the Italian scenario. 相似文献
956.
Richie Poulton Barry J. Milne Michelle G. Craske Ross G. Menzies 《Behaviour research and therapy》2001,39(12):1395-1410
A longitudinal examination of the relation between separation experiences and the development of separation anxiety at age 3, 11 and 18 years was conducted. Three associative pathways (Rachman, S.J. (1978). Fear and courage. San Francisco: W.H. Freeman) were assessed. Conditioning events were not related to separation anxiety at age 3. Vicarious learning (modelling) in middle childhood (age 9 years) was the conditioning variable most strongly related to separation anxiety at age 11, accounting for 1.8% of the variance in symptoms. Separation experiences (hospitalisations) before the age of 9 were inversely correlated with separation anxiety at age 18. That is, more overnight hospital stays in childhood were related to less separation anxiety in late adolescence. However, none of these conditioning correlates remained significant predictors of separation anxiety in adjusted regression models. In contrast, certain “planned” separations in early–mid childhood were associated with lower levels of separation anxiety at later ages. Generally, the findings were consistent with predictions from the non-associative theory of fear acquisition. That vicarious learning processes appeared to modulate, albeit to a minor degree, the expression of separation anxiety during mid–late childhood suggests that there may be critical periods during which some individuals are susceptible to the interactive effects of both associative and non-associative processes. These findings serve to illustrate the complexity of fear acquisition, the relevance of developmental factors and the likely interplay between associative and non-associative processes in the etiology of fear and anxiety. 相似文献
957.
This paper focuses on recent debates over the nature ofliberalism and its central feature of reason, both inside and outside ofeducational philosophy. Central ideas from Jonathan and Hirst contributeas do those from Rawls, Gadamer, Wittgenstein, Taylor, and Ackermantoward a less traditional contextualized and contingent view. 相似文献
958.
Recent attention has focused on establishing school-based integrated services centers which offer educational and noneducational services to students and their families. We present two comparative case studies of client participation in school centers established in low income Latino and Southeast Asian communities. The cases focus on types of center services utilized by families, culturally compatible practices of the centers, and impact of coordinated services on students and families. The data reveal the limited capacity of high poverty families to cope with daily and persistent life challenges and how the schools have been transformed into family support environments which assist families to manage their lives in ways that lead to self-sufficiency and growth. 相似文献
959.
Philosophical Studies - 相似文献
960.
John Predebon 《Psychological research》2001,65(4):226-234
The spatial range of the illusory effects in Müller-Lyer (M-L) figures was examined in three experiments. Experiments 1 and
2 assessed the pattern of bisection errors along the shaft of the standard or double-angle (experiment 1) and the single-angle
(experiment 2) M-L figures: Subjects bisected the shaft and the resulting two half-segments of the shaft to produce apparently
equal quarters, and then each of the quarters to produce eight equal-appearing segments. The bisection judgments of each segment
were referenced to the segment's physical midpoints. The expansion or wings-out and the contraction or wings-in figures yielded
similar patterns of bisection errors. For the standard M-L figures, there were significant errors in bisecting each half,
and each end-quarter, but not the two central quarters of the shaft. For the single-angle M-L figures, there were significant
errors in bisecting the length of the shaft, the half-segment, and the quarter, of the shaft adjacent to the vertex but not
the second quarter from the vertex nor in dividing the half of the shaft at the open end of the figure into four equal intervals.
Experiment 3 assessed the apparent length of the half-segment of the shaft at the open end of the single-angle figures. Length
judgments were unaffected by the vertex at the opposite end of the shaft. Taken together, the results indicate that the length
distortions in both the standard and single-angle M-L figures are not uniformly distributed along the shaft but rather are
confined mainly to the quarters adjacent to the vertices. The present findings imply that theories of the M-L illusion which
assume uniform expansion or contraction of the shafts are incomplete.
Received: 15 December 2000 / Accepted: 5 June 2001 相似文献