全文获取类型
收费全文 | 12760篇 |
免费 | 530篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
13292篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 73篇 |
2020年 | 136篇 |
2019年 | 168篇 |
2018年 | 238篇 |
2017年 | 265篇 |
2016年 | 266篇 |
2015年 | 201篇 |
2014年 | 265篇 |
2013年 | 1439篇 |
2012年 | 457篇 |
2011年 | 496篇 |
2010年 | 308篇 |
2009年 | 322篇 |
2008年 | 488篇 |
2007年 | 442篇 |
2006年 | 435篇 |
2005年 | 435篇 |
2004年 | 438篇 |
2003年 | 421篇 |
2002年 | 448篇 |
2001年 | 223篇 |
2000年 | 187篇 |
1999年 | 199篇 |
1998年 | 212篇 |
1997年 | 204篇 |
1996年 | 163篇 |
1995年 | 174篇 |
1994年 | 161篇 |
1993年 | 179篇 |
1992年 | 168篇 |
1991年 | 141篇 |
1990年 | 143篇 |
1989年 | 127篇 |
1988年 | 120篇 |
1987年 | 131篇 |
1986年 | 120篇 |
1985年 | 147篇 |
1984年 | 188篇 |
1983年 | 191篇 |
1982年 | 192篇 |
1981年 | 174篇 |
1980年 | 141篇 |
1979年 | 132篇 |
1978年 | 177篇 |
1977年 | 156篇 |
1976年 | 160篇 |
1975年 | 141篇 |
1974年 | 159篇 |
1973年 | 107篇 |
1972年 | 76篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
Stephen Pothier John Philbeck David Chichka Daniel A. Gajewski 《Behavior research methods》2009,41(1):107-112
Although there are many well-known forms of visual cues specifying absolute and relative distance, little is known about how visual space perception develops at small temporal scales. How much time does the visual system require to extract the information in the various absolute and relative distance cues? In this article, we describe a system that may be used to address this issue by presenting brief exposures of real, three-dimensional scenes, followed by a masking stimulus. The system is composed of an electronic shutter (a liquid crystal smart window) for exposing the stimulus scene, and a liquid crystal projector coupled with an electromechanical shutter for presenting the masking stimulus. This system can be used in both full- and reduced-cue viewing conditions, under monocular and binocular viewing, and at distances limited only by the testing space. We describe a configuration that may be used for studying the microgenesis of visual space perception in the context of visually directed walking. 相似文献
992.
Distraction by irrelevant background sound of visually-based cognitive tasks illustrates the vulnerability of attentional selectivity across modalities. Four experiments centred on auditory distraction during tests of memory for visually-presented semantic information. Meaningful irrelevant speech disrupted the free recall of semantic category-exemplars more than meaningless irrelevant sound (Experiment 1). This effect was exacerbated when the irrelevant speech was semantically related to the to-be-remembered material (Experiment 2). Importantly, however, these effects of meaningfulness and semantic relatedness were shown to arise only when instructions emphasized recall by category rather than by serial order (Experiments 3 and 4). The results favor a process-oriented, rather than a structural, approach to the breakdown of attentional selectivity and forgetting: performance is impaired by the similarity of process brought to bear on the relevant and irrelevant material, not the similarity in item content. 相似文献
993.
John M. Findlay 《Psychological research》2009,73(2):127-135
The relationship between attention and eye movements is explored by consideration of the variety of ways attention may affect
saccade programming. Four forms of attention are distinguished: one exogenous and three endogenous through either locations
or objects or features. Each of these can control or influence the saccadic generation process. Visual onsets generate a rapid
transient signal with low resolution that travels from the visual system to the oculomotor system. Generation of an eye movement
is associated with an attentional signal, probably also with low resolution, travelling from the oculomotor system to the
visual system. A high-resolution attentional signal, which appears to require voluntary effort, can also select saccadic targets. 相似文献
994.
Research and theorizing on executive function (EF) in childhood has been disproportionately focused on preschool age children. This review paper outlines the importance of examining EF throughout childhood, and even across the lifespan. First, examining EF in older children can address the question of whether EF is a unitary construct. The relations among the EF components, particularly as they are recruited for complex tasks, appear to change over the course of development. Second, much of the development of EF, especially working memory, shifting, and planning, occurs after age 5. Third, important applications of EF research concern the role of school-age children’s EF in various aspects of school performance, as well as social functioning and emotional control. Future research needs to examine a more complete developmental span, from early childhood through late adulthood, in order to address developmental issues adequately. 相似文献
995.
John Lippitt 《International Journal for Philosophy of Religion》2009,65(3):125-138
A philosophical assessment of Richard Dawkins’ The God Delusion, exposing some errors of reasoning that undermine part of the foundation of his atheism. Distinctions between theism, atheism
and agnosticism are also provided and explored for their significance to Dawkins’ argument. 相似文献
996.
997.
Lars Nyberg Ulrich Olofsson John M. Gardiner Lars-Göran Nilsson 《Psychological research》1997,59(4):231-239
The purpose of this study was to assess retrieval strategy in incidental, intentional, and inclusion tests with word-fragment cues following a levels-of-processing manipulation at study. The results of Exp. 1 showed small levels-of-processing effects in incidental tests, and most subjects reported involuntary rather than voluntary retrieval of study-list words. In an intentional test, although levels of processing had a much greater effect, quite a few subjects also reported involuntary rather than voluntary retrieval of study-list words, and these subjects showed a smaller effect of levels of processing than subjects reporting voluntary retrieval. These results suggest that subjects given instructions for both voluntary and involuntary retrieval of study-list words in an inclusion test might not in fact attempt voluntary retrieval at all, but simply adopt an involuntary retrieval strategy. The results of Exp. 2 provided evidence to support this suggestion. The general implication is that where test contamination refers to subjects' failure to use retrieval strategies in accordance with test instructions, inclusion tests can be contaminated, as well as incidental or intentional tests, and that it is always necessary to obtain converging evidence about the actual strategies subjects use. 相似文献
998.
John E. Hunter 《Psychological science》1997,8(1):3-7
Abstract— The significance test as currently used is a disaster Whereas most researchers falsely believe that the significance test has an error rate of 5%, empirical studies show the average error rate across psychology is 60%—12 times higher than researchers think it to be The error rate for inference using the significance test is greater than the error rate using a coin toss to replace the empirical study. The significance test has devastated the research review process. Comprehensive reviews cite conflicting results on almost every issue. Yet quantitatively accurate review of the same results shows that the apparent conflicts stem almost entirely from the high error rate for the significance test. If 60% of studies falsely interpret their primary results, then reviewers who base their reviews on the interpreted study "findings" will have a 100% error rate in concluding that there is conflict between study results. 相似文献
999.
THE SIMILARITY OF BRAIN ACTIVITY ASSOCIATED WITH TRUE AND FALSE RECOGNITION MEMORY DEPENDS ON TEST FORMAT 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
Marcia K. Johnson Scott F. Nolde Mara Mather John Kounios Daniel L. Schacter Tim Curran 《Psychological science》1997,8(3):250-257
Abstract— Event-related potentials (ERPs) were compared for correct recognitions of previously presented words and false recognitions of associatively related, nonpresented words (lures). When the test items were presented blocked by test type (old, new, lure), waveforms for old and lure items were different, especially at frontal and left parietal electrode sites, consistent with previous positron emission tomography (PET) data (Schacter, Reiman, et. al., 1996). When the test format randomly intermixed the types of items, waveforms for old and lure items were more similar. We suggest that test format affects the type of processing subjects engage in, consistent with expectations from the source-monitoring framework (Johnson, Hashtroudi, & Lindsay, 1993). These results also indicate that brain activity as assessed by neuroimaging designs requiring blocked presentation of trials (e g, PET) do not necessarily reflect the brain activity that occurs in cognitive-behavioral paradigms, in which types of test trials are typically intermixed. 相似文献
1000.