首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   42402篇
  免费   1809篇
  国内免费   27篇
  44238篇
  2020年   463篇
  2019年   575篇
  2018年   817篇
  2017年   806篇
  2016年   884篇
  2015年   609篇
  2014年   751篇
  2013年   3731篇
  2012年   1391篇
  2011年   1425篇
  2010年   858篇
  2009年   927篇
  2008年   1269篇
  2007年   1254篇
  2006年   1135篇
  2005年   1047篇
  2004年   1036篇
  2003年   969篇
  2002年   1014篇
  2001年   1245篇
  2000年   1207篇
  1999年   949篇
  1998年   512篇
  1997年   481篇
  1996年   440篇
  1995年   465篇
  1994年   416篇
  1993年   449篇
  1992年   782篇
  1991年   714篇
  1990年   694篇
  1989年   653篇
  1988年   640篇
  1987年   589篇
  1986年   589篇
  1985年   657篇
  1984年   592篇
  1983年   561篇
  1982年   481篇
  1979年   592篇
  1978年   485篇
  1977年   437篇
  1976年   458篇
  1975年   484篇
  1974年   570篇
  1973年   555篇
  1972年   468篇
  1968年   479篇
  1967年   422篇
  1966年   413篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
11.
12.
The authors tested effects of a 10-week group cognitive-behavioral stress management intervention among 100 women newly treated for Stage 0-II breast cancer. The intervention reduced prevalence of moderate depression (which remained relatively stable in the control condition) but did not affect other measures of emotional distress. The intervention also increased participants' reports that having breast cancer had made positive contributions to their lives, and it increased generalized optimism. Both remained significantly elevated at a 3-month follow-up of the intervention. Further analysis revealed that the intervention had its greatest impact on these 2 variables among women who were lowest in optimism at baseline. Discussion centers on the importance of examining positive responses to traumatic events--growth, appreciation of life, shift in priorities, and positive affect-as well as negative responses.  相似文献   
13.
14.
15.
16.
17.
18.
Three experiments, each using a single group of pigeons, are reported. In Experiment 1 subjects were initially trained with two stimuli, one of which was always followed by food, the other being reinforced according to a 50% partial reinforcement schedule. Subsequently a serial procedure was adopted in which an additional stimulus, C, was consistently followed by the partially reinforced CS. A second additional stimulus, A, was followed on half of its occurrences by the continuously reinforced CS, its remaining presentations being followed by nothing. The rate of autoshaped keypecking was substantially greater during A than during C. In the remaining experiments subjects received first-order conditioning with a single stimulus that was either partially (Experiment 2) or continuously (Experiment 3) reinforced. The stimuli A and C were then again introduced for serial autoshaping. Stimulus A was occasionally paired with the CS and occasionally followed by nothing, whereas stimulus C was always followed by the CS. As in Experiment 1, the rate of responding during A was greater than during C. It is proposed that one influence on the rate of autoshaped keypecking during a CS is the accuracy with which the immediate consequences of that CS are predicted.  相似文献   
19.
20.
The three reliability and two construct validity studies presented here represent the first psychometric research on the commercially successful and popular Hartman Color Code Personality Profile. The reliability studies found high reliability after 3 and 6 weeks. The construct validity studies substantiated that the Color Code assesses some personality traits. The magnitude of the error variance suggests caution, however, in using the Color Code classifications at the individual level.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号