首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   12901篇
  免费   533篇
  国内免费   2篇
  2021年   73篇
  2020年   139篇
  2019年   172篇
  2018年   240篇
  2017年   267篇
  2016年   269篇
  2015年   206篇
  2014年   267篇
  2013年   1460篇
  2012年   459篇
  2011年   503篇
  2010年   307篇
  2009年   326篇
  2008年   494篇
  2007年   446篇
  2006年   443篇
  2005年   442篇
  2004年   443篇
  2003年   428篇
  2002年   457篇
  2001年   223篇
  2000年   197篇
  1999年   201篇
  1998年   216篇
  1997年   207篇
  1996年   165篇
  1995年   173篇
  1994年   163篇
  1993年   179篇
  1992年   172篇
  1991年   143篇
  1990年   145篇
  1989年   128篇
  1988年   119篇
  1987年   132篇
  1986年   119篇
  1985年   148篇
  1984年   189篇
  1983年   193篇
  1982年   196篇
  1981年   173篇
  1980年   142篇
  1979年   133篇
  1978年   177篇
  1977年   156篇
  1976年   161篇
  1975年   142篇
  1974年   157篇
  1973年   106篇
  1972年   77篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
11.
12.
13.
Cognitive neuropsychology provides a theoretical framework and methods that can be of value in the study of developmental disorders, but the "dissociation" logic at the centre of this approach is not well suited to the developmental context. This is illustrated with examples from specific language impairment. Within the developing language system there is ample evidence for interaction between levels of representation, with modularity emerging in the course of development. This means that one typically is seeking to explain a complex pattern of associated impairments, rather than highly selective deficits. For instance, a selective impairment in auditory processing can have repercussions through the language system and may lead to distinctive syntactic deficits that are seen in written as well as spoken language. Changes in the nature of representations and in the relationships between components of a developing system mean that cross-sectional data at a single point in development may be misleading indicators of the primary deficit. Furthermore, traditional cognitive neuropsychology places a disproportionate emphasis on representational (competence) deficits, with processing (performance) deficits being relatively neglected. Methods for distinguishing these two kinds of impairment are discussed, as well as other approaches for elucidating the underlying nature of developmental disorders.  相似文献   
14.
15.
16.
17.
18.
The ability of two species of tits to remember the location and/or features of an object was tested in a delayed-matching-to-sample procedure. Three values of retention interval between presentation of the sample stimulus and the choice--30 sec, 5 min, and 15 min--were used. Both species performed at above-chance level at all retention intervals, and there was no significant decline in accuracy with increasing interval. A pool of 100 stimulus objects was used, but the results of control trials indicated that the birds responded primarily to location rather than stimulus features of the object itseif. Although the food-storing marsh tit tended to perform at a higher level than the non-storing great tit, the only significant difference between the species was in the first 50 trials of the first treatment, when the birds were acquiring the task. The results are discussed in relation to the hypothesized special memory capacity of food-storing birds.  相似文献   
19.
Three experiments, each using a single group of pigeons, are reported. In Experiment 1 subjects were initially trained with two stimuli, one of which was always followed by food, the other being reinforced according to a 50% partial reinforcement schedule. Subsequently a serial procedure was adopted in which an additional stimulus, C, was consistently followed by the partially reinforced CS. A second additional stimulus, A, was followed on half of its occurrences by the continuously reinforced CS, its remaining presentations being followed by nothing. The rate of autoshaped keypecking was substantially greater during A than during C. In the remaining experiments subjects received first-order conditioning with a single stimulus that was either partially (Experiment 2) or continuously (Experiment 3) reinforced. The stimuli A and C were then again introduced for serial autoshaping. Stimulus A was occasionally paired with the CS and occasionally followed by nothing, whereas stimulus C was always followed by the CS. As in Experiment 1, the rate of responding during A was greater than during C. It is proposed that one influence on the rate of autoshaped keypecking during a CS is the accuracy with which the immediate consequences of that CS are predicted.  相似文献   
20.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号