首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   57036篇
  免费   1340篇
  国内免费   14篇
  2019年   400篇
  2018年   3892篇
  2017年   3278篇
  2016年   2782篇
  2015年   667篇
  2014年   644篇
  2013年   3236篇
  2012年   1575篇
  2011年   3370篇
  2010年   3088篇
  2009年   2121篇
  2008年   2668篇
  2007年   3043篇
  2006年   906篇
  2005年   1081篇
  2004年   1002篇
  2003年   943篇
  2002年   937篇
  2001年   917篇
  2000年   894篇
  1999年   665篇
  1998年   464篇
  1997年   443篇
  1992年   622篇
  1991年   585篇
  1990年   597篇
  1989年   576篇
  1988年   520篇
  1987年   536篇
  1986年   548篇
  1985年   620篇
  1984年   583篇
  1983年   550篇
  1982年   441篇
  1981年   471篇
  1980年   376篇
  1979年   562篇
  1978年   479篇
  1977年   425篇
  1976年   427篇
  1975年   539篇
  1974年   596篇
  1973年   585篇
  1972年   483篇
  1971年   467篇
  1970年   444篇
  1969年   467篇
  1968年   558篇
  1967年   520篇
  1966年   507篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
41.
42.
43.
44.
The three reliability and two construct validity studies presented here represent the first psychometric research on the commercially successful and popular Hartman Color Code Personality Profile. The reliability studies found high reliability after 3 and 6 weeks. The construct validity studies substantiated that the Color Code assesses some personality traits. The magnitude of the error variance suggests caution, however, in using the Color Code classifications at the individual level.  相似文献   
45.
46.
It is widely assumed that reinforcers are biologically relevant stimuli, or stimuli that have been associated with biologically relevant stimuli. However, brief, arbitrary stimuli have also been reported to have reinforcement-like effects, despite being unrelated to biologically relevant stimuli like food. The present study explored the potential reinforcement-like effects of brief stimuli across 5 experiments. In Experiments 1 through 4, pigeon subjects responded for food reinforcement and brief stimulus presentations in a 2-component multiple schedule. Neither baseline response rates nor resistance to change during disruption tests were systematically greater in a component with versus without brief stimulus presentations. Increasing the rate and duration of brief stimulus presentations in Experiment 4 did not reveal reinforcement-like effects when compared directly with food. In Experiment 5, pigeons chose between independent terminal links in a concurrent-chains procedure. Across conditions, varying the location, duration, and rate of brief stimulus presentations in the terminal links had no systematic effects on preference. In contrast, varying rates of food reinforcers resulted in large and reliable shifts in preference. Therefore, the present study found no systematic evidence that brief stimuli unrelated to food reliably increase response rates, resistance to change, or preference. These data demonstrate the value of systematic replication, and a behavioral momentum approach to assessing potential reinforcement-like effects.  相似文献   
47.
48.
49.
50.
Early explications of family communication patterns (FCP) suggested that a family's communication pattern arises as a result of interactions in which parents and adolescents influence each other. In contrast, empirical research usually treats FCP as parent‐imposed norms that influence socialization outcomes in adolescents, without testing whether adolescents' socialization influences FCP. This study revisits the assumption that adolescents influence FCP, using data from a longitudinal quasi‐experimental evaluation of a school‐based civics curriculum intervention (N = 313 parent‐adolescent pairs). Results show that the intervention‐stimulated adolescents to initiate discussion of politics at home and that these adolescent‐initiated discussions influenced adolescents' and parents' perceptions of FCP 6 months later.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号