全文获取类型
收费全文 | 391篇 |
免费 | 4篇 |
专业分类
395篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 1篇 |
2021年 | 2篇 |
2020年 | 4篇 |
2019年 | 6篇 |
2018年 | 7篇 |
2017年 | 6篇 |
2016年 | 9篇 |
2015年 | 11篇 |
2014年 | 17篇 |
2013年 | 41篇 |
2012年 | 16篇 |
2011年 | 14篇 |
2010年 | 16篇 |
2009年 | 23篇 |
2008年 | 26篇 |
2007年 | 19篇 |
2006年 | 16篇 |
2005年 | 16篇 |
2004年 | 12篇 |
2003年 | 15篇 |
2002年 | 16篇 |
2001年 | 10篇 |
2000年 | 9篇 |
1999年 | 8篇 |
1998年 | 11篇 |
1997年 | 6篇 |
1996年 | 6篇 |
1995年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 7篇 |
1993年 | 6篇 |
1992年 | 7篇 |
1991年 | 5篇 |
1990年 | 2篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 2篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1979年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 1篇 |
1974年 | 1篇 |
1973年 | 3篇 |
1970年 | 1篇 |
1967年 | 1篇 |
1960年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有395条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
Frederik Aust Birk Diedenhofen Sebastian Ullrich Jochen Musch 《Behavior research methods》2013,45(2):527-535
Nonserious answering behavior increases noise and reduces experimental power; it is therefore one of the most important threats to the validity of online research. A simple way to address the problem is to ask respondents about the seriousness of their participation and to exclude self-declared nonserious participants from analysis. To validate this approach, a survey was conducted in the week prior to the German 2009 federal election to the Bundestag. Serious participants answered a number of attitudinal and behavioral questions in a more consistent and predictively valid manner than did nonserious participants. We therefore recommend routinely employing seriousness checks in online surveys to improve data validity. 相似文献
52.
This study investigated the conditions under which the processing in a speeded response task interferes with concurrent processing in a visual encoding task. Three experiments used a dual-task paradigm, in which a speeded left or right response to at one was combined with the identification of a masked left-or right pointing arrow following the tone with variable SOA. Two additional experiments tested the impact of the presentation of pure tone on visual encoding.There were four major findings. First, an unspecific decrease in identification accuracy was observed with decreasing SOA. Second, a blindness to response-compatible stimuli was observed with speeded responses. Third, a specific interference was found between low- and high-pitched tones and left- or right-pointing arrows. Fourth, the specific tone-arrow interference modulated the specific responsearrow interference when the task allowed both to occur simultaneously. The present findings, which suggest both procedural and structural interference between response preparation and stimulus encoding, are discussed in terms of a two-stage model of action planning. 相似文献
53.
54.
Jochen Eckert 《Psychotherapeut》1998,43(4):245-246
Ohne Zusammenfassung 相似文献
55.
An algorithm is described that computes relative frequencies of occurrence of all arbitrarily long substrings of sequential data, such as are obtained from experiments in learning/memory and verbal interaction. The algorithm offers high speed and provides systematization for the computation of empirical conditional probabilities. Use of this algorithm allows application of probabilistic and information theoretic disciplines to reveal dependencies between events separated arbitrarily in time. 相似文献
56.
Jochen Bojanowski 《Philosophia》2016,44(4):1209-1228
Are our actions morally good because we approve of them or are they good independently of our approval? Are we projecting moral values onto the world or do we detect values that are already there? For many these questions don’t state a real alternative but a secular variant of the Euthyphro dilemma: If our actions are good because we approve of them moral goodness appears to be arbitrary. If they are good independently of our approval, it is unclear how we come to know their moral quality and how moral knowledge can be motivating. None of these options seems attractive; the source of moral goodness unclear. Despite the growing literature on Kant’s moral epistemology and moral epistemology the question remains open what Kant’s answer to this apparent dilemma is. The Kantian view I attempt to lay out in this paper is supposed to dissolve the secular version of the Euthyphro dilemma. In responding to this dilemma we need to get clear about the source or the origin of our moral knowledge: Voluntary approval or mind-independent moral facts? Projectivism or detectivism? Construction or given? I believe that all these ways of articulating the problem turn out, on closer inspection, to be false alternatives. 相似文献
57.
We used a concurrent-task paradigm to investigate the attentional cost of simple visual tasks. As in earlier studies, we found that detecting a unique orientation in an array of oriented elements (“pop-out”) carries little or no attentional cost. Surprisingly, this is true at all levels of performance and holds even when pop-out is barely discriminable. We discuss this finding in the context of our previous report that the attentional cost of stimulus detection is strongly influenced by the presence and nature of other stimuli in the display (Braun, 1994b). For discrimination tasks, we obtained a similarly mixed outcome: Discrimination of letter shape carried a high attentional cost whereas discrimination of color and orientation did not. Taken together, these findings lead us to modify our earlier position on the attentional costs of detection and discrimination tasks (Sagi & Julesz, 1985). We now believe that observers enjoy a significant degree of “ambient” visual awareness outside the focus of attention, permitting them to both detect and discriminate certain visual information. We hypothesize that the information in question is selected by a competition for saliency at the level of early vision. 相似文献
58.
59.
60.