全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3597篇 |
免费 | 74篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
3673篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 28篇 |
2023年 | 52篇 |
2022年 | 39篇 |
2021年 | 57篇 |
2020年 | 103篇 |
2019年 | 114篇 |
2018年 | 133篇 |
2017年 | 137篇 |
2016年 | 155篇 |
2015年 | 74篇 |
2014年 | 119篇 |
2013年 | 400篇 |
2012年 | 165篇 |
2011年 | 198篇 |
2010年 | 126篇 |
2009年 | 110篇 |
2008年 | 135篇 |
2007年 | 158篇 |
2006年 | 129篇 |
2005年 | 112篇 |
2004年 | 98篇 |
2003年 | 97篇 |
2002年 | 96篇 |
2001年 | 72篇 |
2000年 | 41篇 |
1999年 | 40篇 |
1998年 | 34篇 |
1997年 | 30篇 |
1996年 | 25篇 |
1995年 | 39篇 |
1994年 | 39篇 |
1993年 | 26篇 |
1992年 | 39篇 |
1991年 | 24篇 |
1990年 | 22篇 |
1989年 | 24篇 |
1988年 | 28篇 |
1987年 | 21篇 |
1986年 | 24篇 |
1985年 | 26篇 |
1984年 | 20篇 |
1983年 | 22篇 |
1982年 | 18篇 |
1980年 | 14篇 |
1979年 | 20篇 |
1978年 | 29篇 |
1977年 | 19篇 |
1975年 | 14篇 |
1974年 | 18篇 |
1973年 | 15篇 |
排序方式: 共有3673条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
991.
This study attempts to replicate an experiment reported by Seamon (1972). In the present investigation, as in the study by Seamon, the scanning of short-term memory was compared when its contents were rehearsed words vs. a mental image. Memory sets were composed of either one, two, or three words. In the relational imagery group, subjects were required to form a single interactive mental scene of the entities which the memory set words represent. Nonimagery subjects were given instructions to covertly rehearse the memory set. In both groups, the usual memory set size (m) effect was obtained, i.e., reaction time (RT) increased linearly with m. Moreover, the set size effect was the same for both groups. This latter finding stands in marked contrast to the result obtained by Seamon; he found no effect of set size when subjects were given interactive imagery instructions. Because of the failure to replicate Seamon, an additional group of subjects were given imagery instructions. For this latter group, some of the procedural discrepancies between the relational imagery group of the present study and the corresponding group in Seamon’s study were resolved. Also, in this additional group, the set size effect was examined as a function of the subjects’ ratings of the quality of the images which they had formed. The same set size effect was found for this additional group, and the effect was independent of image quality. 相似文献
992.
Two kinds of explanations have been offered for the process by which three-term series problems are solved, one in terms of linguistic principles and the other in terms of visual-spatial imagery. Two experiments are reported in which three different classes of operations are brought to bear on the problem: (1) Manipulation of stimulus attributes (characteristics of problems), (2) manipulation of variables that selectively encourage or inhibit the use of imagery (facilitating instructions; the suppression of visualization by reading), and (3) measurement of relevant individual differences (spatial-reasoning ability). All of the results indicate that imagery plays a functional but not a necessary role in the solution of three-term series problems; it is suggested that imaginal representation is functional because it reduces the load on memory. An adequate explanation of problem-solving will have to address certain general issues, such as the diversity of forms of cognitive representation and differences within and between individuals in the choice of problem-solving strategies. 相似文献
993.
Interrater differences in scoring actual WISC protocols were determined for three different IQ levels. In general, differences among the 94 examiners tended to be within an acceptable range as established by the SEm; varince on two Verbal subtests occasionally exceeded their corresponding SEm. Also, examiner's demographic characteristics were not a major consistent source of variance in scoring WISC protocols. 相似文献
994.
995.
996.
The apparent heaviness of a set of 40 cylindrical objects was scaled by the method of magnitude estimation. The objects varied in weight, volume. and density. There were three main conclusions: (1) For any constant volume, heaviness grows as a power function of weight; the larger the volume. the larger the exponent of the power function. The family of such power functions converge at a common point in the vicinity of the heaviest weight that can be lifted. (2) For any constant density (i:e., weight proportional to volume), heaviness does not grow as a power function of weight. (3) For any constant weight, heaviness decreases approximately as a logarithmic function of volume; the constants of the log function depend systematically on the weight of the object. The outcome furnishes a broad quantitative picture of apparent heaviness and of the size-weight illusion (Charpentier’s illusion). 相似文献
997.
998.
Robert B. Cialdini Betty Lee Darby Joyce E. Vincent 《Journal of experimental social psychology》1973,9(6):502-516
A model to account for the positive relationship between transgression and altruism was proposed and tested against three alternative formulations—Guilt, Social Justice, and Self-esteem Bolstering. The proposed model, Negative State Relief, asserts that people in a transgression situation behave charitably in order to reduce a general, negative affective state which is induced by exposure to harm-doing and that if the negative state is relieved by some other means, benevolent activity will be rendered unnecessary. All of the experimental subjects were exposed to an act of transgression; half performed the act themselves while half only witnessed it. In one set of experimental conditions (the relief conditions), a positive event—the receipt of either money or approval—was interposed between the harmful act and a fellow-student's request for aid. In another set of experimental conditions (the norelief conditions), no such positive event occurred. It was found, as predicted, that subjects who received a positive event were significantly less helpful than those who did not, but were not different from a control group which had never been exposed to the transgression. It was also found that harm-doers and harm-witnesses were identical in benevolent tendency. The results were interpreted as favoring a hedonistic conception of the nature of altruism. Finally, it was contended that a U-shaped relationship exists between mood state and helping tendency. 相似文献
999.
Donald Hammil Professor of Education
Libby Goodman
Doctoral StudentJ. Lee Wiederholt
Research Associate 《Journal of School Psychology》1971,9(4):430-435This research dealt with the reliability and validity of the DTVP when used with a sample of economically disadvantaged, predominantly Negro children from a large eastern city. Regarding reliability, test-retest and split-half procedures were employed; for validity the test was correlated with intelligence and achievement measures. The authors concluded that (a) the total test values alone evidence the necessary reliability to be used with confidence for diagnostic purposes, and (b) the validity of the measure has not been sufficiently demonstrated. 相似文献
1000.
A partly occluded visual object is perceptually filled in behind the occluding surface, a process known as amodal completion or visual interpolation. Previous research focused on the image-based properties that lead to amodal completion. In the present experiments, we examined the role of a higher-level visual process-visual short-term memory (VSTM)-in amodal completion. We measured the degree of amodal completion by asking participants to perform an object-based attention task on occluded objects while maintaining either zero or four items in visual working memory. When no items were stored in VSTM, participants completed the occluded objects; when four items were stored in VSTM, amodal completion was halted (Experiment 1). These results were not caused by the influence of VSTM on object-based attention per se (Experiment 2) or by the specific location of to-be-remembered items (Experiment 3). Items held in VSTM interfere with amodal completion, which suggests that amodal completion may not be an informationally encapsulated process, but rather can be affected by high-level visual processes. 相似文献