全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1268篇 |
免费 | 12篇 |
专业分类
1280篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 19篇 |
2019年 | 20篇 |
2018年 | 20篇 |
2017年 | 32篇 |
2016年 | 30篇 |
2015年 | 25篇 |
2014年 | 25篇 |
2013年 | 120篇 |
2012年 | 44篇 |
2011年 | 51篇 |
2010年 | 28篇 |
2009年 | 33篇 |
2008年 | 44篇 |
2007年 | 45篇 |
2006年 | 51篇 |
2005年 | 39篇 |
2004年 | 34篇 |
2003年 | 26篇 |
2002年 | 32篇 |
2001年 | 16篇 |
2000年 | 17篇 |
1999年 | 20篇 |
1998年 | 23篇 |
1997年 | 21篇 |
1996年 | 14篇 |
1995年 | 20篇 |
1994年 | 26篇 |
1993年 | 18篇 |
1992年 | 23篇 |
1991年 | 28篇 |
1990年 | 16篇 |
1989年 | 15篇 |
1988年 | 12篇 |
1987年 | 17篇 |
1986年 | 20篇 |
1985年 | 17篇 |
1984年 | 18篇 |
1983年 | 13篇 |
1982年 | 15篇 |
1981年 | 15篇 |
1980年 | 11篇 |
1979年 | 21篇 |
1978年 | 14篇 |
1977年 | 14篇 |
1976年 | 16篇 |
1975年 | 12篇 |
1974年 | 13篇 |
1973年 | 11篇 |
1971年 | 10篇 |
1968年 | 9篇 |
排序方式: 共有1280条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
201.
Carlson K Dickey MW Frazier L Clifton C 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》2009,62(1):114-139
In English, new information typically appears late in the sentence, as does primary accent. Because of this tendency, perceivers might expect the final constituent or constituents of a sentence to contain informational focus. This expectation should in turn affect how they comprehend focus-sensitive constructions such as ellipsis sentences. Results from four experiments on sluicing sentences (e.g., The mobster implicated the thug, but we can't find out who else) suggest that perceivers do prefer to place focus late in the sentence, though that preference can be mitigated by prosodic information (pitch accents, Experiment 2) or syntactic information (clefted sentences, Experiment 3) indicating that focus is located elsewhere. Furthermore, it is not necessarily the direct object, but the informationally focused constituent that is the preferred antecedent (Experiment 4). Expectations regarding the information structure of a sentence, which are only partly cancellable by means of overt focus markers, may explain persistent biases in ellipsis resolution. 相似文献
202.
Male goalkeepers of intermediate skill level attempted to stop penalty kicks executed with the instep and inside foot, in
situ. A mobile eye tracker and an external camera were used to collect the gaze and motor behaviors of the goalkeepers, as
well as the penalty takers’ motor behaviors and flight of the ball. Percent saves was greater during instep (28%) than inside
foot kicks (12%), but we detected few differences in fixation frequency, location, duration, or transitions that could be
attributed to the type of kick used. Fixation transitions (or the frequency of gaze shifts between locations) were significantly
higher on goals than on saves. During the final phase of the kicking action, the quiet eye was located on the visual pivot
and was longer during saves than goals. Furthermore, when the final fixation on the ball exceeded approximately 1,100 ms,
then the likelihood of goals increased. The results are discussed in light of past studies in goaltending and the dual demands
of motor tasks that require information be fixated both early and late at spatial locations that exceed the limits of focal
vision. 相似文献
203.
The social networking site (SNS) Facebook is becoming increasingly recognized as a medium through which individuals can investigate and monitor others' activities. However, little is known about whether Facebook monitoring behavior occurs within romantic relationships and, accordingly, the psychological predictors of this behavior. The present study employed an extended theory of planned behavior (TPB) framework including self-esteem, partner trust, and demographic characteristics, to predict frequent Facebook partner-monitoring. Facebook users (N=244) in romantic relationships completed measures assessing the standard TPB constructs (attitude, subjective norm, and perceived behavioral control), additional predictor variables (self-esteem and partner trust), and demographic characteristics (age, gender, relationship length, daily Facebook logins, and time spent per login). One week later, participants reported their level of Facebook partner-monitoring during the previous week. Regression analyses supported the standard TPB constructs of attitude and subjective norm in predicting intentions to engage in frequent Facebook partner-monitoring, with intention, in turn, predicting behavior. Partner trust, but not self-esteem, significantly predicted frequent Facebook partner-monitoring intentions. Of the demographic characteristics, daily Facebook logins significantly predicted both intention and behavior and, unexpectedly, relationship length directly affected behavior. Overall, the current study revealed that frequent Facebook partner-monitoring is influenced by attitudinal, normative, and relational factors and, potentially, increased visits to Facebook. These findings provide a new understanding of an individual's use of the world's leading SNS to monitor their partner's activities and provide a foundation for future studies to investigate the potential negative implications this activity may have for those in romantic relationships. 相似文献
204.
Memory &; Cognition - Co-reference occurs when two or more noun phrases refer to the same individual, as in the following inferential problem: Mark is kneeling by the fire or he is looking at... 相似文献
205.
This paper reports the results of two experiments. In the first, voice-key naming times were collected and in the second, keypress naming times were collected for 250 of the Snodgrass and Vanderwart (1980) pictures. The resulting naming times and correct naming rates were well predicted in multiple regression analyses by one or another measure of codability (name or concept agreement) and by age-of-acquisition ratings collected specifically for this study. Voice-key responses appeared to be somewhat more sensitive indicators of naming difficulty, although keypress responses did remarkably well. The Appendix presents the age-of-acquisition ratings, the trimmed vocal and keypress naming times, and the correct naming rates from the two experiments for the 250 pictures. 相似文献
206.
Walsh V 《Trends in cognitive sciences》2003,7(11):483-488
Research into the perception of space, time and quantity has generated three separate literatures. That number can be represented spatially is, of course, well accepted and forms a basis for research into spatial aspects of numerical processing. Links between number and time or between space and time, on the other hand, are rarely discussed and the shared properties of all three systems have not been considered. I propose here that time, space and quantity are part of a generalized magnitude system. I outline A Theory Of Magnitude (ATOM) as a conceptually new framework within which to re-interpret the cortical processing of these elements of the environment. 相似文献
207.
Louise M. Baca Joan D. Koss-Chioino 《Journal of multicultural counseling and development》1997,25(2):130-141
This article discusses a group counseling model specifically targeted for Mexican American adolescents with behavior problems including substance abuse. An integrated model is proposed, with the model assumptions clearly articulated and specific therapeutic goals addressed in a culturally responsive manner. Foci of the model are ethnic identity, affective expression, and life choices. Preliminary findings indicate that the youths are retained in the groups at a higher rate than predicted. To live in the Borderlands means you are neither hispana India negra espanola ni gabacha, eres mestiza, mulata, half-breed caught in the crossfire between camps while carrying all five races on your back not knowing which side to turn to, run from; To survive the Borderlands you must live sin fronteras be a crossroads. —Gloria Anzaldua (1987) 相似文献
208.
One hundred thirty-four participants completed a revised Menstrual Distress Questionnaire (MDQ), which included criteria for premenstrual dysphoric disorder (PMDD). Two weeks later the participants again completed the revised MDQ after reading either the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994) diagnostic criteria for the PMDD or a copy of the same criteria retitled Episodic Dysphoric Disorder, with all menstrual cycle references removed. Knowledge of the diagnosis did not affect women's perceptions of their own menstrual cycle-related symptoms, but it increased participants' perceptions of premenstrual changes as a problem for women in general. Chi-square analyses revealed that participants were more willing to attach a psychiatric diagnosis to women they know if they believed the diagnosis was related to the menstrual cycle. 相似文献
209.
210.
Joan McCord 《Behavior research methods》1986,18(2):210-213
In the spring of 1983, Drexel University announced that all incoming freshmen the following fall would be required to have access to a personal computer being designed by Apple. This paper reports on the impact of that decision over the next 2 years. 相似文献