全文获取类型
收费全文 | 721篇 |
免费 | 19篇 |
专业分类
740篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 8篇 |
2023年 | 5篇 |
2022年 | 6篇 |
2021年 | 16篇 |
2020年 | 15篇 |
2019年 | 16篇 |
2018年 | 33篇 |
2017年 | 29篇 |
2016年 | 37篇 |
2015年 | 33篇 |
2014年 | 30篇 |
2013年 | 66篇 |
2012年 | 34篇 |
2011年 | 32篇 |
2010年 | 18篇 |
2009年 | 16篇 |
2008年 | 36篇 |
2007年 | 23篇 |
2006年 | 17篇 |
2005年 | 20篇 |
2004年 | 12篇 |
2003年 | 26篇 |
2002年 | 23篇 |
2001年 | 13篇 |
2000年 | 11篇 |
1999年 | 4篇 |
1998年 | 13篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1996年 | 7篇 |
1995年 | 9篇 |
1994年 | 6篇 |
1993年 | 5篇 |
1992年 | 5篇 |
1991年 | 6篇 |
1990年 | 8篇 |
1989年 | 9篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1986年 | 5篇 |
1985年 | 5篇 |
1984年 | 5篇 |
1983年 | 6篇 |
1982年 | 5篇 |
1981年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 8篇 |
1978年 | 7篇 |
1977年 | 6篇 |
1972年 | 3篇 |
1971年 | 3篇 |
1968年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有740条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
141.
Jo Anne Tracy Ph.D. Richard M. McFall Joseph E. Steinmetz 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》2005,40(1):45-54
Using a classical eyeblink conditioning paradigm, we have previously shown that the rate of acquisition of a conditioned response
may be manipulated by engaging subjects in background tasks of varying complexity concurrent to conditioning. To further examine
the influence of the background environment on conditioning, a picture set designed to elicit emotional responses, the International
Affective Picture System (IAPS), was presented to subjects during classical eyeblink conditioning. The results suggest that
eyeblink conditioning does appear to be sensitive to contextual manipulations of arousal. Pictures rated as very arousing
were found to engage subjects enough to enhance learning, although not to the point that autonomic functions were significantly
altered between picture groups. We suggest that group differences in learning may be a result of either novelty of, or vigilance
to, interesting pictures rather than as a direct result of physiological arousal. 相似文献
142.
Bruce N. Waller 《Philosophia》2004,31(3-4):427-447
143.
Nicolas?FavezEmail author Chloé?Lavanchy?Scaiola Hervé?Tissot Jo?lle?Darwiche France?Frascarolo 《Journal of child and family studies》2011,20(1):23-37
We present the first steps in the validation of an observational tool for father-mother-infant interactions: the FAAS (Family
Alliance Assessment Scales). Family-level variables are acknowledged as unique contributors to the understanding of the socio-affective
development of the child, yet producing reliable assessments of family-level interactions poses a methodological challenge.
There is, therefore, a clear need for a validated and clinically relevant tool. This validation study has been carried out
on three samples: one non-referred sample, of families taking part in a study on the transition to parenthood (normative sample;
n = 30), one referred for medically assisted procreation (infertility sample; n = 30) and one referred for a psychiatric condition in one parent (clinical sample; n = 15). Results show that the FAAS scales have (1) good inter-rater reliability and (2) good validity, as assessed through
known-group validity by comparing the three samples and through concurrent validity by checking family interactions against
parents’ self-reported marital satisfaction. 相似文献
144.
Vinícius Ynoe de Moraes Miguel Roberto Jorge Flávio Faloppa João Carlos Belloti 《Journal of clinical psychology in medical settings》2010,17(1):31-37
There are few studies on the development of anxiety and depression in orthopaedics and trauma (O&T) inpatients. We designed
a cross-sectional study aimed at comparing the prevalence of depression and anxiety in 100 O&T inpatients and 100 clinical
inpatients in the same hospital. O&T patients were divided into subgroups: trauma and non-trauma (arthroplasty, tumour, and
infection sub grouping). We measured anxiety and depression by the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale and co-morbidities
by the Charlson age-adjusted comorbidity index (CCI). For the trauma subgroup, AO/OTA fracture classification and Gustillo
and Anderson grade of open fractures classification was applied. The prevalence of anxiety and depression was 35% and 28%, respectively for the clinical sample, and 44% and 33% for the O&T
sample. Compared with the clinical sample, anxiety scores were higher in the O&T sample (p = .047), and in arthroplasty (p = .020) and trauma subgroups (p = .031). In the O&T sample, high CCI scores were associated with high anxiety scores (p = .033). 相似文献
145.
Angelo Loula Ricardo Gudwin Charbel Niño El-Hani João Queiroz 《Cognitive Systems Research》2010,11(2):131-147
In this paper, we describe a digital scenario where we simulated the emergence of self-organized symbol-based communication among artificial creatures inhabiting a virtual world of unpredictable predatory events. In our experiment, creatures are autonomous agents that learn symbolic relations in an unsupervised manner, with no explicit feedback, and are able to engage in dynamical and autonomous communicative interactions with other creatures, even simultaneously. In order to synthesize a behavioral ecology and infer the minimum organizational constraints for the design of our creatures, we examined the well-studied case of communication in vervet monkeys. Our results show that the creatures, assuming the role of sign users and learners, behave collectively as a complex adaptive system, where self-organized communicative interactions play a major role in the emergence of symbol-based communication. We also strive in this paper for a careful use of the theoretical concepts involved, including the concepts of symbol and emergence, and we make use of a multi-level model for explaining the emergence of symbols in semiotic systems as a basis for the interpretation of inter-level relationships in the semiotic processes we are studying. 相似文献
146.
This study examined the precompetition temporal patterning of competitive anxiety components in 42 athletes with disability who participated at the national level and at the national trials for the Paralympic Games in a variety of sports. All subjects completed a modified version of the Competitive State Anxiety Inventory-2 which measures intensity and direction of the competitive anxiety response on three occasions before competition (1 wk., 2 hr., and 20 min.). Analysis suggested that for cognitive and somatic dimensions athletes with disabilities show a similar precompetition anxiety response to athletes without disability. However, there appear to be some differences, particularly in the intensity of self-confidence, as athletes with disability reported a reduction of self-confidence just prior to competition. 相似文献
147.
148.
Goldberg [Goldberg, L. R.(in press) Doing it all Bass-Ackwards: The development of hierarchical factor structures from the top down. Journal of Research in Personality] has recently described a novel method for computing hierarchical component structures via a “top down” design. He calls his method the “Bass Ackwards” approach to distinguish it from more common “bottom up” methods for obtaining hierarchical solutions. Using simple matrix equations, in this commentary I demonstrate that the end result of a Bass Ackwards analysis—that is, the correlations between component scores from different levels of a hierarchy—can be calculated without calculating the actual component scores. By avoiding component-score calculation, Goldberg’s method (a) can be applied to any data set in which a correlation matrix is available, and (b) can be applied to factor scores rather than estimated factor scores. Goldberg suggests that the Bass Ackwards method represents a useful tool for elucidating the underlying dimensionality and latent structure (rotation in hyperspace) of a data set. Computer code is reported in the Appendix A of this article to help personality researchers more fully evaluate this claim in empirical and simulated data. 相似文献
149.
150.