首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   647篇
  免费   82篇
  国内免费   117篇
  2024年   2篇
  2023年   16篇
  2022年   19篇
  2021年   26篇
  2020年   41篇
  2019年   29篇
  2018年   44篇
  2017年   30篇
  2016年   29篇
  2015年   21篇
  2014年   23篇
  2013年   50篇
  2012年   46篇
  2011年   51篇
  2010年   51篇
  2009年   36篇
  2008年   49篇
  2007年   59篇
  2006年   60篇
  2005年   54篇
  2004年   28篇
  2003年   16篇
  2002年   11篇
  2001年   4篇
  2000年   4篇
  1999年   9篇
  1998年   4篇
  1997年   4篇
  1996年   5篇
  1995年   2篇
  1993年   1篇
  1992年   3篇
  1991年   2篇
  1989年   5篇
  1988年   3篇
  1987年   1篇
  1986年   3篇
  1983年   2篇
  1982年   1篇
  1980年   2篇
排序方式: 共有846条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
赵文洪 《学海》2011,(2):158-165
中世纪的欧洲公地共同体公共事务的管理,一定程度上体现着民主、平等和法治的精神:民主精神体现在共同体全体成员广泛直接参与庄园法庭和村民会议的立法、决策、司法活动以及选举和担任共同事务管理者;平等精神体现于共同体成员公共权利价值的平等、在村规面前的平等;在管理共同事务的资格方面的平等。法治精神体现于法律至上、法律面前人人平等、重视法律程序。民主平等法治精神,是公地制度留给后世的政治学遗产。  相似文献   
62.
海克尔在《宇宙之谜》*第十七章"科学和基督教"中除了破题指出科学和基督教之间的矛盾日益尖锐化,是动荡的十九世纪的显著特征之一外,  相似文献   
63.
一、养生的最高境界———和养生之道的最高境界是达到“和”的状态。养生得和是在与外物交接中保持内心平和,静默恬淡,做到物欲与情欲不患于心,神、形、气、物兼养而不伤的自身和谐的理想境界。养者,养神为本,养形为末。本者,天也,根也,原也;末者,逐也,随也。养神与养形互为本  相似文献   
64.
宗教本质新论   总被引:7,自引:0,他引:7  
本文对现代意义上的宗教学诞生以来的各种宗教本质观作了回顾,着重探讨了对我国学界影响较大的几种宗教本质观之间的分歧及其各自的理论缺陷,提出了"宗教是一种特殊形态的精神货币,宗教起源于人类为满足自己物质和精神需要所进行的人与异己力量之间的特殊的交换活动"的宗教本质观与宗教起源说。并对宗教的核心─-信仰问题发表了独到的见解,认为,宗教的本质是中性的,无褒贬之分,宗教的积极意义与消极意义是宗教与政治挂钩的产物。  相似文献   
65.
In this study, a priming Stroop paradigm was used to determine whether stereotype activation is unintentional. Priming conditions (priming/no‐priming) and the relationship between priming and target (consistent/inconsistent/no‐relation) were the independent variables; accuracy, reaction time and N400 amplitude were used as dependent variables. The reaction time revealed that stereotype activation is, to some extent, unintentional. Furthermore, the event‐related potenial (ERP) results showed that N400 amplitude was larger for inconsistent conditions than for consistent conditions. This result supported the notion that stereotype activation is an unintentional and automatic process.  相似文献   
66.
Previous studies have found that when low‐status group members are aware that their in‐group is stereotyped as dependent by a specific out‐group (i.e. a dependency meta‐stereotype is salient), they are reluctant to seek help from the high‐status out‐group to avoid confirming the negative meta‐stereotype. However, it is unclear whether low‐status group members would seek more help in the context of a salient dependency meta‐stereotype when there is low (vs. high) group boundary permeability. Therefore, we conducted two experiments to examine the moderating effect of permeability on meta‐stereotype confirmation with a real group. In study 1, we manipulated the salience of the dependency meta‐stereotype, measured participants' perceived permeability and examined their help‐seeking behaviour in a real‐world task. Participants who perceived low permeability sought more help when the meta‐stereotype was salient (vs. not salient), whereas participants who perceived high permeability sought the same amount of help across conditions. In study 2, we manipulated the permeability levels and measured the dependency meta‐stereotype. Participants who endorsed a high‐dependency meta‐stereotype sought more help than participants who endorsed a low‐dependency meta‐stereotype; this effect was particularly strong in the low‐permeability condition. The implications of these results for social mobility and intergroup helping are discussed.  相似文献   
67.
The Cigarette Purchase Task is a behavioral economic assessment tool designed to measure the relative reinforcing efficacy of cigarette smoking across different prices. An exponential demand equation has become a standard model for analyzing purchase task data, but its utility is compromised by its inability to accommodate values of zero consumption. We propose a two‐part mixed effects model that keeps the same exponential demand equation for modeling nonzero consumption values, while providing a logistic regression for the binary outcome of zero versus nonzero consumption. Therefore, the proposed model can accommodate zero consumption values and retain the features of the exponential demand equation at the same time. As a byproduct, the logistic regression component of the proposed model provides a new demand index, the “derived breakpoint”, for the price above which a subject is more likely to be abstinent than to be smoking. We apply the proposed model to data collected at baseline from college students (N = 1,217) enrolled in a randomized clinical trial utilizing financial incentives to motivate tobacco cessation. Monte Carlo simulations showed that the proposed model provides better fits than an existing model. We note that the proposed methodology is applicable to other purchase task data, for example, drugs of abuse.  相似文献   
68.
The purpose of this study was to explore how environmental factors (family environment and school education) and individual characteristics (personality, creative attitudes, and divergent thinking) collectively affect creative achievement of American and Chinese college students. Data were collected from 378 college students in the United States (N = 193) and China (N = 185). Results showed that the US sample had significantly higher means in home resources, high-school education, Conscientiousness, creative achievement, and all four dimensions of the Abbreviated Torrance Test for Adults (ATTA) than the China sample. In contrast, the China sample scored significantly higher in understanding the importance of creativity. Results indicated creative attitudes and divergent thinking sequentially mediated the effects of parental values on creative achievement for the US sample, whereas Openness mediated the effect of high-school education on creative achievement for the China sample. For both samples, creative attitudes mediated the effect of Openness on divergent thinking. The results suggested cultural differences in the effects of environmental factors on creativity, yet more similar findings in the effects of individual characteristics on creativity between the two samples. The results and implications are discussed.  相似文献   
69.
Family theory has refocused attention from deficits to family strengths, and interest in family resilience across cultures has become more widespread. However, few instruments were designed for quantitative research on family resilience. The Family Resilience Assessment Scale (FRAS) was designed to measure family resilience and has been administered in Western populations. The aim of this study was to adapt the FRAS for use with Chinese populations and examine its psychometric properties. The study was conducted using a sample of 991 university students in China (62.9 % female, mean age 20, SD = 1.12). First, the scale was translated and modified into Chinese, and good language equivalency was found. Next, confirmatory factor analysis was used to identify the best-fitting model. Results showed that the 3-factor model consisting of 32 items was an adequate fit. The goodness-of-fit index values for the model were χ 2/df = 5.97, CFI = .84, RMSEA = .08, SRMR = .05. Total scale reliability was α = .95. According to these values it can be said that the modified Chinese version of the FRAS was adapted successfully to the Chinese culture. Moreover, researchers and clinicians can use this instrument to assess family strengths when working with individuals in families experiencing adversity.  相似文献   
70.
We investigated the association between cultural worldviews and climate change risk perceptions, support for climate friendly policies and climate change mitigation behaviours in a large Chinese sample. Items from Dake's cultural theory scales and Kahan's cultural cognition scale were presented to a Qualtrics online panel consisting of 515 Mandarin‐speaking residents of Beijing. A series of factor analyses revealed that the combined item sets were best represented by four‐dimensions: hierarchism, individualism, egalitarianism and fatalism. Mediation analysis revealed that respondents with egalitarian and non‐fatalist worldviews perceived greater risk associated with climate change, which in turn predicted greater support for policies to manage climate change and increased mitigation behaviour. In addition, respondents who scored high on individualism were less likely to support climate change policies, but this effect was not mediated by risk perceptions. Overall, our results suggest cultural worldviews may influence policy support both directly and indirectly through risk perceptions.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号