首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   40篇
  免费   7篇
  2023年   2篇
  2022年   1篇
  2021年   2篇
  2020年   1篇
  2019年   4篇
  2018年   2篇
  2017年   2篇
  2016年   3篇
  2014年   1篇
  2013年   6篇
  2011年   1篇
  2010年   1篇
  2009年   3篇
  2008年   3篇
  2006年   3篇
  2004年   1篇
  2003年   1篇
  2002年   3篇
  2001年   1篇
  2000年   1篇
  1998年   2篇
  1997年   1篇
  1996年   1篇
  1992年   1篇
排序方式: 共有47条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
31.
32.
Training behavioral technicians mainly focuses on teaching accurate implementation of structured behavioral intervention programs. Often behavioral technicians are unable to adequately promote their clients' learning in less structured environments, which can limit opportunities for generalization of the clients' skills to the natural environment. The aim of the present study was to evaluate the effectiveness of using behavioral skills training to coach behavioral technicians on the implementation of naturalistic behavioral interventions. Naturalistic behavioral interventions take advantage of naturally occurring situations to teach new skills and practice mastered skills in natural settings, thus promoting generalization across environments and in the presence of natural contingencies. Five behavioral technicians were trained to implement a novel protocol based on play therapy. Specifically, they were coached to engage in well‐defined positive behaviors during their interactions with clients (e.g., labeled praise). All participants reached mastery criteria, maintained skills at follow‐up, and demonstrated generalization of skills with novel clients.  相似文献   
33.
A clinical phenomenology of the concept ‘unconscious fantasy’ attempts to describe it from a ‘bottom‐up’ perspective, that is, from the immediate experience of the analyst working in session. Articles of psychoanalytic authors from different persuasions are reviewed, which taken as a whole would shed some light on how the concept of unconscious fantasy takes shape in the analyst's mind during the session with the patient. A clinical phenomenology in three steps is described. Each step is illustrated by clinical material. Current controversies around the concept of unconscious fantasy (or phantasy) are still trapped in the discussion about if and how they are really unconscious. The strategy to describe from a ‘bottom‐up’ perspective the process of how the analyst's mind embraces the idea that an emerging phenomenon in the relationship with the patient can be defined as ‘unconscious fantasy’, allows us to elude the question as to whether or not we believe that unconscious fantasies exist at all, since we are neither required to assert or deny such a prior existence in order to describe the process of elaboration which, in the end, does formulate a fantasy as fantasy.  相似文献   
34.
It is widely believed that a descending serial circuit consisting of neural projections from the basolateral complex (BLA) to the central nucleus (CEA) of the amygdala mediates fear expression. Here we directly test this hypothesis and show that disconnecting the BLA and CEA with asymmetric neurotoxic lesions after Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats completely abolishes the expression of conditional freezing. These results demonstrate that neural projections from the BLA to CEA are essential for the expression of learned fear responses.Long-standing anatomical models of the brain circuitry underlying learned fear posit serial flow of information through the amygdala to engage the expression of fear responses (LeDoux 2000; Maren 2001). Specifically, conditioning-induced plasticity in the lateral nucleus of the amygdala (LA) is thought to drive learned fear through excitatory axonal projections to the basal nuclei of the amygdala (BA; basolateral and basomedial nuclei), which in turn send unidirectional and excitatory synaptic projections to the medial division of the central nucleus of the amygdala (CEm). Neurons in the CEm project to brain structures involved in the production of a variety of fear responses (Pitkänen et al. 1997; Swanson and Petrovich 1998). Alternatively, neurons in LA might excite neurons in CEm by limiting inhibitory input from the intercalated cell masses (ITC) interposed between the basolateral complex (BLA; lateral and basal nuclei) and the central nucleus of the amygdala (CEA) (Paré et al. 2004). In either case, the BLA is well positioned to drive learned fear responses via anatomical connections with the CEA.Although an extensive literature demonstrates the importance of both the BLA and CEA in the acquisition and expression of fear (LeDoux et al. 1990; Lee et al. 1996; Amorapanth et al. 2000; Goosens and Maren 2001), it is not known whether a functional connection between the two structures is essential for the expression of learned fear. Indeed, the BLA and CEA make independent contributions to aversively motivated learning under some conditions (Killcross et al. 1997; Amorapanth et al. 2000). Moreover, recent work in appetitive conditioning paradigms challenges the necessity of serial circuits in the amygdala for associative learning processes (Holland and Gallagher 1999; Everitt et al. 2003; Balleine and Killcross 2006). It is therefore essential to determine whether serial connections between the BLA and CEA are involved in the expression of fear memories as widely assumed in the literature.To address this issue, we made asymmetric neurotoxic lesions of the BLA and CEA after Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats. That is, we placed BLA lesions in one hemisphere and CEA lesions in the contralateral hemisphere, thereby producing a functional disconnection of the two brain regions. Control animals received neurotoxic BLA and CEA lesions in the same hemisphere, thereby leaving both structures and their connections intact in one hemisphere. This disconnection strategy (e.g., Olton et al. 1982) capitalizes on the fact that projections from the BLA to CEA are both ipsilateral and unidirectional (Pitkänen et al. 1997). It has been used by several groups to assess the contribution of connections between brain areas to learning and memory processes, including conditioned stimulus processing (Han et al. 1999) and appetitive spatial learning (Ito et al. 2008), for example.Fear conditioning was conducted in standard observation chambers (see Supplemental Methods) and consisted of five pairings of an auditory conditional stimulus (CS) (2 kHz, 10 sec, 80 dB) with a footshock unconditioned stimulus (US) (2 sec, 1 mA); the intertrial interval (ITI) was 1 min. Freezing behavior served as the measure of conditional fear. Twenty-four hours after fear conditioning, the rats were deeply anesthetized, and amygdala lesions were made by infusing N-methyl-d-aspartate (NMDA; 20 mg/mL in 100 mM phosphate buffered saline with a pH 7.4; Sigma) into the CEA and BLA through a 28-g injection cannula attached to a Hamilton syringe via polyethylene tubing. Control rats received unilateral lesions of the BLA and CEA in the same hemisphere (IPSI, n = 13) or sham surgery (SHAM, n = 16), whereas experimental rats received the unilateral BLA and CEA lesions in opposite hemispheres (CONTRA, n = 11). Hence, functional connectivity between the BLA and CEA was left intact in one hemisphere among rats in the IPSI group, whereas the asymmetric lesions in rats in the CONTRA group eliminated this connection. Retention tests assessing fear to the conditioning context and tone CS were conducted in separate sessions one week after recovery from surgery. Histological examination of coronal brain sections obtained from the subjects after the experiment revealed selective CEA and BLA lesions in each group; representative lesions are illustrated in Figure 1, A and B. The lesions spared fibers of passage (Fig. 1C).Open in a separate windowFigure 1.Disconnection of the BLA and CEA impairs the expression of conditional fear. (A) Schematic representation of a typical BLA (shaded) and CEA (black) lesions among rats with unilateral lesions placed in the same hemisphere (IPSI) or in opposite hemispheres (CONTRA). (B) Photomicrograph of a thionin-stained coronal section from a representative rat in the CONTRA group. The image has been cropped to include only the left and right amygdale. Broken lines encircle CEA and BLA lesions in the left and right hemispheres, respectively. (C, left) Photomicrograph of a thionin-stained coronal section from a representative rat in the IPSI group. The image has been cropped to focus on the lesion in the right amygdala. Broken lines encircle the CEA and BLA lesions. (C, right) Photomicrograph of an AuCl-stained coronal section adjacent to that shown at left indicates that NMDA infusions into the amygdala did not damage myelinated axons in the vicinity of the lesion. (D) Mean percentage of freezing (± SEM) during the conditioning session (collapsed across CSs and ITIs) and retention tests in rats in each of the three groups. The expression of fear to the conditioning context and tone CS was severely impaired by disconnection of the BLA and CEA in the CONTRA group. *P < 0.05.As shown in Figure 1D (left panel), all rats acquired similar levels of conditional freezing (collapsed across the CS and ITI for each trial) by the end of the presurgical fear conditioning session (main effect of trial, F(5,185) = 42.1, P < 0.0001); there was neither a main effect of group (F < 1) nor a group × trial interaction (F < 1.3). One week after recovery from surgery, conditional freezing to the conditioning context and the auditory CS was assessed in separate retention tests. Importantly, functional disconnection of the BLA and the CEA after fear conditioning severely impaired the expression of conditioned freezing to both the conditioning context and the auditory CS (Fig. 1D, right). Indeed, the expression of fear to the auditory CS was essentially abolished. Freezing among rats in the CONTRA group was significantly lower than that in both the SHAM and IPSI groups during both retention tests (main effect of group, F(2,37) = 9.98, P < 0.001), and this difference did not interact with the nature of the retention test (F < 1). Hence, functional disconnection of the BLA and CEA produced an impairment in the expression of conditional fear that was much greater than that produced by comparable lesions that spared this connection.The logic of the disconnection procedure requires that focal CEA and BLA lesions in each hemisphere do not damage the adjacent BLA or CEA, respectively. For instance, if CEA lesions produce retrograde degeneration in the neighboring BLA, rats in the CONTRA group would effectively have bilateral lesions in the BLA. Such an outcome would be expected to yield the massive deficits in conditional freezing that we have observed. Inspection of thionin-stained coronal sections suggested that, as in previous studies (Goosens and Maren 2001), our lesions were indeed selective for the targeted areas (Fig. 1B). However, to increase our confidence that the BLA adjacent to a CEA lesion was in fact functional, we performed c-fos immunohistochemistry on brain sections obtained from a separate group of rats trained and tested as previously described (SHAM, CONTRA, and IPSI groups; n = 8 per group). In addition to these rats, we included a group of rats that were not conditioned (NO-SHOCK, n = 8) to quantify fear-induced increases in c-fos expression. All rats were sacrificed 90 min after the retention test to the auditory CS. Amygdaloid c-fos expression in the SHAM rats and the intact hemisphere of the IPSI rats did not differ, and these groups were therefore collapsed into a single SHAM group.As shown in Figure 2, SHAM rats exhibited a greater density of c-fos positive nuclei in both the CEA and BLA relative to nonshocked controls. Quantification of these data confirmed this observation and revealed significant differences in c-fos expression among the groups in both the CEA (Fig. 2, left; F(2,40) = 5.59, P < 0.01) and the BLA (Fig. 2, right; F(2,40) = 5.07, P < 0.01). Importantly, BLA c-fos expression adjacent to CEA lesions in the CONTRA group was similar to that in intact SHAM rats, and significantly greater than that in nonshocked controls (Fig. 2B, right). This suggests that the failure of CONTRA rats to express conditional fear responses was not due to a failure to engage the intact BLA but rather to the functional disconnection of BLA activity from CEA output. Indeed, the CEA adjacent to a BLA lesion exhibited a marked reduction in c-fos expression relative to SHAM controls (Fig. 2, left), indicating that BLA lesions failed to drive ipsilateral CEA neurons important for the expression of learned fear. It is possible that the absence of c-fos expression in the CEA, in this case, is due to encroachment of the adjacent BLA lesion. However, thionin-stained sections revealed that BLA lesions were selective. Moreover, we observed intact BLA c-fos expression in rats with adjacent CEA lesions suggesting that nearby lesions per se do not disrupt c-fos expression.Open in a separate windowFigure 2.Functional disconnection of the BLA and CEA revealed by c-fos expression. Mean density (± SEM) of c-fos positive nuclei in the CEA (left) and BLA (right) in rats from each of the three groups. Disconnection of the BLA and CEA eliminated fear-related increases in c-fos expression in the CEA (left), but not the BLA (right). *P < 0.05.These results reveal that a functional connection between the BLA and CEA is required for the expression of learned fear. Considering that BLA projections to the CEA are largely unidirectional, our data reveal that a serial circuit from the BLA to the CEA mediates the expression of conditional fear responses. Consistent with this view, there are numerous reports that permanent lesions or reversible inactivation of either the BLA or CEA prevent the expression of conditioned fear (Lee et al. 1996; Maren 1999; Zimmerman et al. 2007). It is now apparent that the necessity for both the BLA and CEA in fear expression arises from the functional connectivity between them. Anatomically, this connection might involve a direct excitatory projection from BA to CEm (Paré et al. 1999) or indirect projections from LA to ITC and the lateral division of the central nucleus (CEl), both of which project to CEm (Smith and Paré 1994; Paré et al. 2004). Recent data reveal, however, that selective immunotoxic lesions of the ITC do not impair the expression of conditioned freezing (Likhtik et al. 2008). Moreover, CEl projections to CEm are inhibitory, making it unlikely that that an LA-CEl projection drives learned fear responses via CEm (Paré et al. 2004). Hence, it appears that the most likely route by which fear CSs drive learned fear involves projections from BA to CEm. Consistent with this, selective BA lesions disrupt the expression of conditioned freezing when made either before (Goosens and Maren 2001) or after (Anglada-Figueroa and Quirk 2005) fear conditioning.The dependence of conditional fear on a serial circuit between the BLA and CEA stands in contrast to the independent roles these areas have been proposed to play in appetitive conditioning paradigms (Holland and Gallagher 1999; Everitt et al. 2003; Balleine and Killcross 2006). For example, CEA, but not BLA, lesions have been reported to produce deficits in the acquisition and expression of autoshaped conditioned responses (CRs), indicating that the CEA has an independent contribution to CR expression for food-motivated responses (Parkinson et al. 2000). Moreover, the BLA has a role in the attribution of incentive salience to rewarding stimuli independent of the generation of CRs to those stimuli (Hatfield et al. 1996). However, in aversive conditioning, it appears that the BLA may not encode the motivational properties of the shock US (Rabinak and Maren 2008), but rather CS–US associations that are essential for organizing conditional fear responses by the CEA. Indeed, there is an emerging body of data suggesting that these associations may be established not only in sensory afferents in the BLA but also in the CEA (Wilensky et al. 2006; Zimmerman et al. 2007). Synaptic plasticity in projections from BA to CEm may be the essential substrate underlying the functional connectivity between these structures that is essential for the expression of fear memory.  相似文献   
35.
This paper addresses the relationship between basic numerical processes and higher level numerical abilities in normal achieving adults. In the first experiment we inferred the elementary numerical abilities of university students from the time they needed to encode numerical information involved in complex additions and subtractions. We interpreted the shorter encoding times in good arithmetic problem solvers as revealing clearer or more accessible representations of numbers. The second experiment shows that these results cannot be due to the fact that lower skilled individuals experience more maths anxiety or put more cognitive efforts into calculations than do higher skilled individuals. Moreover, the third experiment involving non-numerical information supports the hypothesis that these interindividual differences are specific to number processing. The possible causal relationships between basic and higher level numerical abilities are discussed.  相似文献   
36.
37.
In a series of experiments, the consequences of a single and double social conflict on various behaviours and body weight in rats were studied. Animals were subjected to social defeat by placing them into the territory of an aggressive male conspecific for one hour, either once, or twice at the same time on two consecutive days. To assess the consequences of social defeat, three experiments were performed with independent groups of rats. In the first experiment, an open field test was performed two days after the last conflict. Locomotor activity was strongly reduced after social defeat. There were no differences between the single and double defeat group. To assess the effects of social defeat on subsequent social behaviour, a second experiment was performed in which experimental animals were confronted with an unfamiliar non-aggressive rat two days after a single or double conflict. Social defeat resulted in a reduction of social contact with the unfamiliar conspecific. There was no difference between the single and double conflict group. In the third experiment, the effects of social conflict on food intake, body weight and saccharine preference were measured. Food intake was not affected after a single conflict, but in the double conflict group food intake was decreased for several days. Body weight gain was decreased after both single and double social defeat. The decrease was stronger in the double conflict group. Water intake and saccharine preference were not significantly affected. This study revealed that social defeat in rats causes pronounced changes in various behaviours and body weight. Different aspects of behaviour are differentially affected by defeat with respect to the magnitude and time course of the changes induced. Moreover, different behavioural parameters are differentially sensitive to repetition of the stressor.  相似文献   
38.
The present study examines ethnicity as a moderator variable between spiritual well-being (SWB) and psychological and behavioral outcomes. Participants included in this analysis were 88 African American (46.6%) and 101 non-African American (total N = 189) homeless mothers. Through structured interviews conducted at 3-month intervals over a period of 15 months, data were collected on spiritual well-being, mental health, trauma symptoms, substance use, parenting, and child behavior. Hierarchical linear model and general estimating equation results indicated that ethnicity moderates the relationship between SWB and anxiety, trauma symptoms, child behavior, and parenting outcomes. On average, African Americans reported significantly higher SWB than non-African Americans, indicating the relative importance of spirituality in their lives. These findings support previous research indicating a difference in the role that SWB plays in the lives of African Americans compared to non-African Americans.  相似文献   
39.
The purpose of this study was to determine the association between the occurrence of stressful life events and internalizing and externalizing problems, and to analyze longitudinally buffering effects of supportive family relationships. To this end, 100 Spanish adolescents were studied twice, when they were in mid-adolescence (15-16 years) and two years later. They completed questionnaires regarding stressful life events, family relationships, and adolescent adjustment. Results showed that high quality parent-adolescent relationships protected boys and girls against the negative consequences of stressful life events on externalizing, but not internalizing, symptoms. The adolescents who enjoyed good relationships with their parents in mid-adolescence did not increase their externalizing problems in late adolescence as consequence of the occurrence of stressful events. However, these stressors did lead to an increase in the number of externalizing problems when the family relationships were of a middle or low quality. These results highlight the important role that supportive family relationships play in the behavioral adjustment of adolescents, protecting them against some negative consequences of stressful life events, and suggest the relevance of supporting parents through resources such as parent education in order to help them to improve their relationships with their adolescents.  相似文献   
40.
This longitudinal study of the effects of iron deficiency in infancy assessed motor development over time in 185 healthy Costa Rican children who varied in iron status at 12-23 months. Longitudinal analyses (hierarchical linear modeling) used the Bayley Psychomotor Index before and both 1 week and 3 months after iron treatment in infancy and the Bruninks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency - long form at 5 years and short form at 11-14 years. Children with chronic severe iron deficiency in infancy had lower motor scores at the beginning of the study and a lower but parallel trajectory for motor scores through early adolescence. Thus, there was no evidence of catch-up in motor development, despite iron therapy in infancy that corrected iron deficiency anemia in all cases.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号