全文获取类型
收费全文 | 38958篇 |
免费 | 611篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
39571篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 441篇 |
2019年 | 554篇 |
2018年 | 735篇 |
2017年 | 803篇 |
2016年 | 818篇 |
2015年 | 573篇 |
2014年 | 698篇 |
2013年 | 2962篇 |
2012年 | 1272篇 |
2011年 | 1268篇 |
2010年 | 842篇 |
2009年 | 781篇 |
2008年 | 1146篇 |
2007年 | 1153篇 |
2006年 | 1024篇 |
2005年 | 916篇 |
2004年 | 853篇 |
2003年 | 811篇 |
2002年 | 823篇 |
2001年 | 1170篇 |
2000年 | 1124篇 |
1999年 | 882篇 |
1998年 | 386篇 |
1997年 | 378篇 |
1995年 | 362篇 |
1994年 | 352篇 |
1993年 | 356篇 |
1992年 | 736篇 |
1991年 | 654篇 |
1990年 | 703篇 |
1989年 | 607篇 |
1988年 | 647篇 |
1987年 | 601篇 |
1986年 | 607篇 |
1985年 | 549篇 |
1984年 | 507篇 |
1983年 | 484篇 |
1982年 | 353篇 |
1979年 | 572篇 |
1978年 | 402篇 |
1975年 | 455篇 |
1974年 | 496篇 |
1973年 | 532篇 |
1972年 | 399篇 |
1971年 | 389篇 |
1970年 | 351篇 |
1969年 | 402篇 |
1968年 | 472篇 |
1967年 | 429篇 |
1966年 | 354篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
881.
Howard L. Kaplan 《Behavior research methods》1977,9(2):176-183
The existing ISA-standard FORTRAN task-scheduling calls require careful programming to provide timing accuracy even in a single-user system, and do not explicitly provide a mechanism for resolving task conflicts in a multiprogramming system. A simple assignment of priorities to tasks can guarantee timing accuracy to one task, but the risks generating maximum, unnecessary interference with other tasks. An alternative to such a standard priority scheme, not known to be available on common commercial systems, guarantees timing for one task while minimizing interference among tasks. 相似文献
882.
One of two circles on a rotating disk appears to execute a planetary motion about the other circle. It is shown that the fixated circle serves as the center of rotation for the nonfixated circle. The effect of fixation is absent when polygons replace the circles. However, when one corner of an isolated square is fixated, the remaining corners rotated about the fixated corner. These planetary effects are consistent with the retinal paths followed by the elements of a display during fixation. This is not unlike the failure of position constancy associated with smooth pursuit of linearly moving targets in environments lacking a stationary visual frame of reference. In the present instance, however, the “retinal” responses occur during tracking of circularly moving targets, even in the presence of a visual frame of reference. These results are discussed in relation to the stereokinetic phenomenon. It is also shown that there is a strong interaction between the effects of fixation and the configurational features of the display. When a circle and a square are overlapping on a rotating disk, fixation of the square does not produce the perception of planetary motion. However, when the circle is fixated, the square is readily perceived as executing a planetary path about the circle. The possibility that position constancy in general is attributable to the geometry of the scene rather than to a “discounting” of information about eye movements is mentioned. 相似文献
883.
When temporal auditory acuity is estimated using time-reversed click pairs, the asymmetry of temporal masking may influence the results. To investigate this hypothesis, we first established normal-hearing listeners’ psychometric functions for the discrimination of time-reversed click pairs. An attempt to relate listeners’ performances in click-pair discrimination to their performances in forward and backward masking tasks was unsuccessful, probably due to the presence of spectral artifact in the temporal masking paradigms. We then investigated listeners’ abilities to discriminate time-reversed click pairs in which the intensity of the lower level click in one pair was altered. This alteration was introduced at random in order to minimize the influence of spectral artifact. Our results suggest that the course of temporal masking influenced the discriminability of the modified click pairs. Thus, the discrimination of time-reversed click pairs may yield a biased estimate of auditory temporal acuity. 相似文献
884.
A theory of intelligence is couched in stimulus-response terms, bridging the gap between S-R and cognitive psychology. The chief theoretical concepts are span ability (a capacity notion), response string (sequentially cued responses), and complexity of stimulus control (task complexity). Span is equated with the ability to respond appropriately when several cues are conjunctively relevant for correct performance (complex stimulus control). So defined, span is consistently and broadly related to many aspects of intelligent behavior. 相似文献
885.
The use of complementary metal oxide semiconductor (CMOS) technology to produce fixed-ratio and random-ratio schedules is described. Such a technology implies several advantages over traditional methods of construction, especially with respect to cost and complexity. Suitable interfaces for linking with everyday laboratory equipment are also described. 相似文献
886.
887.
Robert B. Welch David J. Schroeder Janet K. Thurgate Carlton K. Erickson Raymond L. Higgins John S. Wait 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1977,21(4):352-356
Ten male and 10 female subjects, half of whom were classified as “heavy” and half as “light” drinkers, ingested a 4-1 combination of tonic water and 80-proof vodka in an amount calculated to produce a state of moderate intoxication (average estimated peak blood alcohol level=.075). This resulted in an increased sensitivity (latency, magnitude, and duration) to the calorically induced “oculogyral illusion” (OGI) relative to a placebo (tonic water) condition. “Heavy” and “light” drinkers did not differ significantly on any of the measures. It was speculated that the alcoholic enhancement of the OGI was due either to a decreased ability to suppress vestibular nystagmus when attempting to fixate a visual stimulus or to the expenditure of extra-normal effort in order to maintain fixation. 相似文献
888.
Experienced observers were asked to identify, in a four-level 2AFC situation, the longer of two unfilled time intervals, each of which was marked by a pair of 20-msec acoustic pulses. When all the markers were identical, high-level (186-dB SPL) bursts of coherently gated sinusoids or bursts of band-limited Gaussian noise, a change in the spectrum of the markers generally did not affect performance. On the other hand, for 1-kHz tone-burst markers, intensity decreases below 25 dB SL were accompanied by sizable deterioration of the discrimination performance, especially at short (25-msec) base intervals. Similarly large changes in performance were observed also when the two tonal markers of each interval were made very dissimilar from each other, either in frequency (frequency difference larger than 1 octave) or in intensity (level of the first marker at least 45 dB below the level of the second marker). Time-difference thresholds in these two latter cases were found to be nonmonotonically related to the base interval, the minima occurring between 40- and 80-msec onset separations. 相似文献
889.
Marilyn L. Shaw 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1977,21(1):15-25
Reaction times were measured in a task which requires the subject to search a brief visual array for a critical letter embedded in a row of background letters. In Experiment 1, the position of a critical letter in an instructed reading order and the size of a set of memorized letters were varied. Mean reaction time increased monotonically with distance of the critical letter from the beginning of the instructed reading path in the display. The variables reading position and memory set size were additive in their effect on mean reaction time. Data from a second experiment in which the retinal location of the critical letter and its reading position were varied showed that both reading position and retinal location influenced mean reaction time, but the effect of reading position on reaction time was greater. These variables interacted. 相似文献
890.