首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   966篇
  免费   45篇
  2023年   9篇
  2022年   7篇
  2021年   13篇
  2020年   16篇
  2019年   11篇
  2018年   28篇
  2017年   25篇
  2016年   26篇
  2015年   17篇
  2014年   25篇
  2013年   121篇
  2012年   43篇
  2011年   43篇
  2010年   32篇
  2009年   25篇
  2008年   41篇
  2007年   35篇
  2006年   36篇
  2005年   40篇
  2004年   29篇
  2003年   29篇
  2002年   22篇
  2001年   16篇
  2000年   25篇
  1999年   19篇
  1998年   15篇
  1997年   9篇
  1996年   11篇
  1995年   9篇
  1994年   24篇
  1993年   11篇
  1992年   10篇
  1991年   12篇
  1990年   7篇
  1989年   11篇
  1988年   15篇
  1987年   12篇
  1986年   6篇
  1985年   5篇
  1984年   9篇
  1982年   6篇
  1981年   6篇
  1980年   5篇
  1979年   7篇
  1978年   5篇
  1975年   7篇
  1974年   5篇
  1970年   5篇
  1967年   8篇
  1966年   5篇
排序方式: 共有1011条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
992.
993.
We hypothesise that the effectiveness of threats and encouragements is contingent on the intended recipient's level of negative affect, as evidenced by his/her negative affective display. Therefore, bargainers can be more effective if, as they make offers, they condition any threats or encouragements on the recipient's affective display. We test this hypothesis using 5561 verbal exchanges that occurred during 192 telephone conversations between credit collectors and debtors. Collectors were most effective in motivating debtors to discuss terms to resolve their debt if they: (1) threatened recipients who were nonresponsive and did not show any negative affect; and (2) encouraged recipients who displayed negative affect. This result suggests that making threats and encouragements contingent on a recipient's displays of negative affect may be an important but frequently overlooked component of bargaining.  相似文献   
994.
995.
996.
997.
Researchers have long been interested in the origins of humans’ understanding of symbolic number, focusing primarily on how children learn the meanings of number words (e.g., “one”, “two”, etc.). However, recent evidence indicates that children learn the meanings of number gestures before learning number words. In the present set of experiments, we ask whether children's early knowledge of number gestures resembles their knowledge of nonsymbolic number. In four experiments, we show that preschool children (n = 139 in total; age M = 4.14 years, SD = 0.71, range = 2.75–6.20) do not view number gestures in the same the way that they view nonsymbolic representations of quantity (i.e., arrays of shapes), which opens the door for the possibility that young children view number gestures as symbolic, as adults and older children do. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at https://youtu.be/WtVziFN1yuI

Highlights

  • Children were more accurate when enumerating briefly-presented number gestures than arrays of shapes, with a shallower decline in accuracy as quantities increased.
  • We replicated this finding with arrays of shapes that were organized into neat, dice-like configurations (compared to the random configurations used in Experiment 1).
  • The advantage in enumerating briefly-presented number gestures was evident before children had learned the cardinal principle.
  • When gestures were digitally altered to pit handshape configuration against number of fingers extended, children overwhelmingly based their responses on handshape configuration.
  相似文献   
998.
999.
1000.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号