全文获取类型
收费全文 | 623篇 |
免费 | 85篇 |
国内免费 | 31篇 |
专业分类
739篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 11篇 |
2023年 | 18篇 |
2022年 | 18篇 |
2021年 | 13篇 |
2020年 | 29篇 |
2019年 | 21篇 |
2018年 | 27篇 |
2017年 | 17篇 |
2016年 | 32篇 |
2015年 | 24篇 |
2014年 | 28篇 |
2013年 | 49篇 |
2012年 | 29篇 |
2011年 | 49篇 |
2010年 | 33篇 |
2009年 | 37篇 |
2008年 | 57篇 |
2007年 | 52篇 |
2006年 | 49篇 |
2005年 | 47篇 |
2004年 | 33篇 |
2003年 | 13篇 |
2002年 | 10篇 |
2001年 | 9篇 |
2000年 | 3篇 |
1999年 | 11篇 |
1998年 | 5篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 1篇 |
1993年 | 3篇 |
1990年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有739条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
41.
大学生上网行为、态度和人格特征的研究 总被引:17,自引:0,他引:17
本研究使用自编大学生上网成瘾诊断问卷、自编上网行为和态度问卷以及三个人格量表,对上海市6所高校370名大学生的上网情况进行了调查。结果表明:(1)上网成瘾的大学生在上网的行为和态度上明显不同于非成瘾的大学生,他们表现出高开放性、社交性、胜任能力和匿名性,并且更认可网络交流的便利性。(2)上网成瘾的大学生比非成瘾的大学生明显表现出孤独、抑郁和焦虑的人格特质。(3)逐步回归分析的结果表明,“社交性”、“交流便利”、“焦虑量表分数”、“开放性”、“胜任能力”对大学生陷人网络的程度有显著的预测作用。 相似文献
42.
青年罪犯的人格特征研究 总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6
采用“卡特尔十六种人格因素测验”问卷,分析比较了青年罪犯与青年学生的人格特质、次元人格因素、应用人格因素。结果表明:青年罪犯与青年学生在8种人格特质(C、F、G、H、L、Q1、Q2、q)、1种次元人格因素(X2)、1种应用人格因素(Y4)上存在显著差异,提示青年罪犯确有异于同龄人的人格特征,这对青年犯罪的预防与矫治具有重要意义。 相似文献
43.
44.
对“家属签字”制度的立法思考 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
邵将 《医学与哲学(人文社会医学版)》2008,29(5):42-44
李丽云悲剧的发生,使《医疗机构管理条例》第三十三条规定的“家属签字”制度成为公众及法律界人士关注的焦点。“家属签字”制度受到道德和法律的双重拷问。从“家属签字”制度中潜在的法律推定出发,对“家属签字”制度的正当性依据、“特殊情况”下对患者的求生推定等六方面问题做些有益探讨,以期完善我国医疗管理法律法规。 相似文献
45.
Humans are limited in their ability to maintain multiple attentional foci. In attentive tracking of moving objects, performance
declines as the number of tracked targets increases. Previous studies have interpreted such reduction in terms of a limit
in the number of attentional foci. However, increasing the number of targets usually reduces spatial separation among different
targets. In this study, we examine the role of target spatial separation in maintaining multiple attentional foci. Results
from a multiple-object tracking task show that tracking accuracy deteriorates as the spatial separation between targets decreases.
We propose that local interaction between nearby attentional foci modulates the resolution of attention, and that capacity
limitation from attentive tracking originates in part from limitations in maintaining critical spacing among multiple attentional
foci. These findings are consistent with the hypothesis that tracking performance is limited not primarily by a number of
locations, but by factors such as the spacing and speed of the targets and distractors. 相似文献
46.
Yuhong Jiang Marvin M. Chun 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology》2001,54(4):1105-1124
The effect of selective attention on implicit learning was tested in four experiments using the contextual cueing paradigm (Chun & Jiang, 1998, 1999). Observers performed visual search through items presented in an attended colour (e.g., red) and an ignored colour (e.g., green). When the spatial configuration of items in the attended colour was invariant and was consistently paired with a target location, visual search was facilitated, showing contextual cueing (Experiments 1, 3, and 4). In contrast, repeating and pairing the configuration of the ignored items with the target location resulted in no contextual cueing (Experiments 2 and 4). We conclude that implicit learning is robust only when relevant, predictive information is selectively attended. 相似文献
47.
Makovski T Sussman R Jiang YV 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2008,34(2):369-380
Given a changing visual environment, and the limited capacity of visual working memory (VWM), the contents of VWM must be in constant flux. Using a change detection task, the authors show that VWM is subject to obligatory updating in the face of new information. Change detection performance is enhanced when the item that may change is retrospectively cued 1 s after memory encoding and 0.5 s before testing. The retro-cue benefit cannot be explained by memory decay or by a reduction in interference from other items held in VWM. Rather, orienting attention to a single memory item makes VWM more resistant to interference from the test probe. The authors conclude that the content of VWM is volatile unless it receives focused attention, and that the standard change detection task underestimates VWM capacity. 相似文献
48.
Previous research has shown that visual search performance is modulated by the current contents in visual working memory (VWM), even when the contents of VWM are irrelevant to the search task. For example, visual search is faster when the target--rather than a distractor--is surrounded by a shape currently held in VWM. This study uses the modulation of visual search by VWM to investigate properties of VWM. Participants wereasked to remember the color or the shape of novel polygons whose "goodness" of figure varied according to Garner's (1962) rotation and reflection transformation principle. During the memory retention interval, participants searched for a tilted line among vertical lines embedded inside colored polygons. Search was faster when the target--rather than a distractor--was enclosed by the remembered polygons. The congruity effect diminished with increasing memory load and decreasing figure goodness. We conclude that congruity effects in visual search can indirectly assess VWM representation strength. 相似文献
49.
50.