全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1567篇 |
免费 | 133篇 |
专业分类
1700篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 4篇 |
2023年 | 29篇 |
2022年 | 26篇 |
2021年 | 35篇 |
2020年 | 69篇 |
2019年 | 74篇 |
2018年 | 98篇 |
2017年 | 101篇 |
2016年 | 121篇 |
2015年 | 73篇 |
2014年 | 88篇 |
2013年 | 195篇 |
2012年 | 119篇 |
2011年 | 130篇 |
2010年 | 58篇 |
2009年 | 72篇 |
2008年 | 73篇 |
2007年 | 75篇 |
2006年 | 50篇 |
2005年 | 58篇 |
2004年 | 40篇 |
2003年 | 43篇 |
2002年 | 20篇 |
2001年 | 6篇 |
2000年 | 8篇 |
1999年 | 9篇 |
1998年 | 3篇 |
1997年 | 5篇 |
1996年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 1篇 |
1994年 | 3篇 |
1990年 | 2篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 1篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有1700条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
Penelope K. Morrison Elizabeth P. Miller Jessica Burke Patricia Cluss Rhonda Fleming Lynn Hawker 《Journal of aggression, maltreatment & trauma》2018,27(2):179-198
As part of a 2-year ethnographic study, 49 adult men who had been convicted of intimate partner violence (IPV) perpetration were interviewed on what they thought would have prevented them from engaging in perpetration behaviors, or rather what they believed prevention with young boys needed to include in order to prevent future IPV perpetration. The authors’ open-ended interviews yielded five main themes: (a) messages about healthy relationship behaviors, (b) the need to promote respect for women, (c) teaching effective skills for communicating and managing anger, (d) programs that provide role models and are school-based, and (e) addressing the impact of experiencing violence as a child. The authors conclude that prevention efforts focused on changing both attitudes regarding IPV and behaviors might be most effective for reducing perpetration, and that greater attention needs to be paid towards prevention among youth most at risk of IPV perpetration, those experiencing violence in their homes. 相似文献
992.
Jessica Rames‐LaPointe Michael D. Hixson Larissa N. Niec Katrina N. Rhymer 《Behavioral Interventions》2014,29(3):253-267
Errorless compliance training (ECT) is a success‐based, nonaversive intervention to reduce child noncompliance that has been shown to be effective in both home and special education settings. In the current study, ECT was evaluated in a multiple‐baseline across‐subjects design with three kindergarten students who displayed noncompliant behavior in their general education classrooms. The researcher delivered a range of classroom requests to each student to determine the probability of compliance for each request. Requests were then arranged in a hierarchy, ranging from those initially yielding high compliance probabilities (level 1) to those yielding low compliance (level 4). At the beginning of treatment, students were presented with a high number of level 1 requests and provided verbal praise for compliance. Over several weeks, lower probability requests were faded in. The three students demonstrated considerable improvement in compliance levels during and following treatment. Improved compliance generalized to low‐probability requests not used in treatment. When the teacher delivered requests, results of generalization were mixed. The results provide preliminary support for the use of errorless compliance training in the general education setting. Copyright © 2014 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
993.
Covered in stigma? The impact of differing levels of Islamic head‐covering on explicit and implicit biases toward Muslim women 下载免费PDF全文
Jim A.C. Everett Fabian M.H. Schellhaas Brian D. Earp Victoria Ando Jessica Memarzia Cesare V. Parise Benjamin Fell Miles Hewstone 《Journal of applied social psychology》2015,45(2):90-104
Given the prominence of Muslim veils—in particular the hijab and full‐face veil—in public discourse concerning the place of Muslims in Western society, we examined their impact on non‐Muslims’ responses at both explicit and implicit levels. Results revealed that responses were more negative toward any veil compared with no veil, and more negative toward the full‐face veil relative to the hijab: for emotions felt toward veiled women (Study 1), for non‐affective attitudinal responses (Study 2), and for implicit negative attitudes revealed through response latency measures (Studies 3a and 3b). Finally, we manipulated the perceived reasons for wearing a veil, finding that exposure to positive reasons for wearing a veil led to better predicted and imagined contact (Study 4). Practical and theoretical implications are discussed. 相似文献
994.
Geoffrey B. Maddox Mary A. Pyc Zachary S. Kauffman Jessica D. Gatewood Aubrey M. Schonhoff 《Memory & cognition》2018,46(8):1376-1388
Although substantial evidence indicates that spacing repeated study events with intervening material generally enhances memory performance relative to massing study events, the mechanism underlying this benefit is less clear. Two experiments examined the role of reminding difficulty during the acquisition of material in modulating final memory performance for spaced repetitions utilizing recognition (Experiment 1) and recall tests (Experiment 2). Specifically, participants studied a list of words presented one or two times separated by one or five items. On each trial participants reported whether the item had been previously presented (i.e., repetition detection judgment), and the response latency served as a proxy for reminding difficulty such that longer response latencies reflected more difficult reminding. A third experiment extended this paradigm with the inclusion of a massed condition and novel lag conditions (three and ten items). Results revealed significant lag effects in final test performance across experiments despite comparable repetition detection difficulty between lag conditions during acquisition. Moreover, results from within-participant point-biserial analyses and mediation analyses converged on overall performance measures in suggesting that repetition detection difficulty failed to modulate final test performance in the current paradigm. Discussion considers the implications of the current results for mechanisms proposed to underlie the benefits of spaced study and spaced retrieval practice. 相似文献
995.
Danice L. Brown Jeremy D. Jewell Amy L. Stevens Jessica D. Crawford Ronald Thompson 《Journal of child and family studies》2012,21(3):359-367
We investigated the relationship between gender and clinician diagnosis of a depressive disorder at intake on variables reflecting
depression among adolescents in residential treatment. It was hypothesized that females diagnosed with a depressive disorder
would have the highest scores on measures of suicide risk, the number of symptoms of a major depressive episode, suicidal
behavior, and suicidal ideation. Results indicated partial support for the predicted comparisons. Females diagnosed with a
depressive disorder scored significantly higher on measures of depressive symptom count and suicidal ideation. Surprisingly,
females without a diagnosis of a depressive disorder had higher suicide risk scores than the remaining groups. The results
of this study highlight the importance of assessment and treatment of internalizing disorders in youth entering residential
programs, particularly female adolescents. 相似文献
996.
Though self-esteem is known to positively impact individuals’ romantic relationship outcomes and those of their partners, the interactive nature of both partners’ self-esteem levels has not been systematically investigated. Using actor-partner interdependence model analyses we estimated actor, partner, and four types of dyadic effects of self-esteem on relationship quality in a sample of over 500 heterosexual dating couples. Lower self-esteem individuals and their partners reported lower satisfaction and commitment to their relationships than did higher self-esteem individuals and their partners. An additive effect whereby both partners’ self-esteem levels combine to predict relationship quality best described the dyadic effects. Results highlight the importance of considering the interactive nature of individual characteristics in dyadic research. 相似文献
997.
Infant responding to joint attention, executive processes, and self-regulation in preschool children
Vaughan Van Hecke A Mundy P Block JJ Delgado CE Parlade MV Pomares YB Hobson JA 《Infant behavior & development》2012,35(2):303-311
Infant joint attention is related to behavioral and social outcomes, as well as language in childhood. Recent research and theory suggests that the relations between joint attention and social-behavioral outcomes may reflect the role of executive self-regulatory processes in the development of joint attention. To test this hypothesis two studies were conducted. The first, cross-sectional study examined the development of responding to joint attention (RJA) skill in terms of increasing executive efficiency of responding between 9 and 18 months of age. The results indicated that development of RJA was characterized by a decreased latency to shift attention in following another person's gaze and head turn, as well as an increase in the proportion of correct RJA responses exhibited by older infants. The second study examined the longitudinal relations between 12-month measures of responding to joint attention and 36-month attention regulation in a delay of gratification task. The results indicated that responding to joint attention at 12-months was significantly related to children's use of three types of self-regulation behaviors while waiting for a snack reward at 36 months of age. These observations are discussed in light of a developmental theory of attention regulation and joint attention in infancy. 相似文献
998.
The presence of binge eating behavior in early middle school predicts future diagnoses and health difficulties. We showed that this early binge eating behavior can be predicted by risk factors assessed in elementary school. We tested the acquired preparedness model of risk, which involves transactions among personality, psychosocial learning, and binge eating. In a sample of 1,906 children assessed in the spring of fifth grade (the last year of elementary school), the fall of sixth grade, and the spring of sixth grade, we found that fifth grade negative urgency (the personality tendency to act rashly when distressed) predicted subsequent increases in the expectancy that eating helps alleviate negative affect, which in turn predicted subsequent increases in binge eating behavior. This transactional risk process appeared to continue to occur at later time points. Negative urgency in the fall of sixth grade was predicted by fifth grade pubertal onset, binge eating behavior, and expectancies. In turn, it predicted increases in high-risk eating expectancies by the spring of sixth grade, and thus heightened risk. 相似文献
999.
A brief wakeful rest after new verbal learning enhances memory for several minutes. In the research reported here, we explored the possibility of extending this rest-induced memory enhancement over much longer periods. Participants were presented with two stories; one story was followed by a 10-min period of wakeful resting, and the other was followed by a 10-min period during which participants played a spot-the-difference game. In Experiment 1, wakeful resting led to significant enhancement of memory after a 15- to 30-min period and also after 7 days. In Experiment 2, this striking enhancement of memory 7 days after learning was demonstrated even when no retrievals were imposed in the interim. The degree to which people can remember prose after 7 days is significantly affected by the cognitive activity that they engage in shortly after new learning takes place. We propose that wakeful resting after new learning allows new memory traces to be consolidated better and hence to be retained for much longer. 相似文献
1000.
David P. Costanza Jessica M. Badger Rebecca L. Fraser Jamie B. Severt Paul A. Gade 《Journal of business and psychology》2012,27(4):375-394