首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   169047篇
  免费   7621篇
  国内免费   155篇
  2021年   1500篇
  2020年   2776篇
  2019年   3433篇
  2018年   3431篇
  2017年   3893篇
  2016年   4566篇
  2015年   3908篇
  2014年   4787篇
  2013年   23403篇
  2012年   4371篇
  2011年   3402篇
  2010年   3791篇
  2009年   4703篇
  2008年   3651篇
  2007年   3181篇
  2006年   3841篇
  2005年   3849篇
  2004年   3342篇
  2003年   2976篇
  2002年   2833篇
  2001年   3104篇
  2000年   2993篇
  1999年   3028篇
  1998年   2810篇
  1997年   2644篇
  1996年   2558篇
  1995年   2395篇
  1994年   2383篇
  1993年   2310篇
  1992年   2522篇
  1991年   2343篇
  1990年   2222篇
  1989年   2155篇
  1988年   2117篇
  1987年   2137篇
  1986年   2129篇
  1985年   2341篇
  1984年   2474篇
  1983年   2258篇
  1982年   2370篇
  1981年   2324篇
  1980年   2157篇
  1979年   2158篇
  1978年   2158篇
  1977年   2132篇
  1976年   1905篇
  1975年   1967篇
  1974年   2036篇
  1973年   1866篇
  1972年   1495篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
181.
Human communication is thoroughly context bound. We present two experiments investigating the importance of the shared context, that is, the amount of knowledge two interlocutors have in common, for the successful emergence and use of novel conventions. Using a referential communication task where black‐and‐white pictorial symbols are used to convey colors, pairs of participants build shared conventions peculiar to their dyad without experimenter feedback, relying purely on ostensive‐inferential communication. Both experiments demonstrate that access to the visual context promotes more successful communication. Importantly, success improves cumulatively, supporting the view that pairs establish conventional ways of using the symbols to communicate. Furthermore, Experiment 2 suggests that dyads with access to the visual context successfully adapt the conventions built for one color space to another color space, unlike dyads lacking it. In linking experimental pragmatics with language evolution, the study illustrates the benefits of exploring the emergence of linguistic conventions using an ostensive‐inferential model of communication.  相似文献   
182.
183.
Do the conditions under which promises are made determine whether they ought to be kept? Philosophers have placed a number of conditions on promising which, they hold, must be met in order to make promise-keeping obligatory. In so doing, they have distinguished valid promises from invalid promises and justified promises from promises that are not justified. Considering such conditions, one by one, we argue that they are mistaken. In the first place, the conditions they lay down are not necessary for either valid or justified promise-making. In the second place, promises need not meet such conditions in order to create moral obligations. In general, such analyses of promising fail because they suffer from a confusion between promise-making and promise-keeping. Philosophers have wrongly supposed that obligations to keep promises are dependent upon, or derivable from, the quality of the promises themselves, at the time they are made, instead of focusing on conditions that must be satisfied at the time when promises are supposed to be kept. It is not the quality of a promise that determines an obligation to keep it but the rightness or wrongness of performing the promised act.  相似文献   
184.
Ohne ZusammenfassungIn dankbarer Erinnerung an meinen verehrten Münchener Lehrer Kurt Huber, dessen frühzeitigen gewaltsamen Tod (13. 7. 43) wir noch immer beklagen. Die Arbeit entsprang einer seiner letzten wissenschaftlichen Anregungen.  相似文献   
185.
186.
Wray  K. Brad 《Synthese》2019,196(3):819-832
Synthese - Hoyningen-Huene (Systematicity: the nature of science, Oxford University Press, Oxford, 2013) develops an account of what science is, distinguishing it from common sense. According to...  相似文献   
187.
188.
189.
190.
Are different responses differentially associable with their consequences? An overshadowing design was used to try to answer this question for three responses previously studied in golden hamsters. In Experiment I, scrabbling was rapidly suppressed by electric shock punishment, and it overshadowed a tone which occurred between scrabbling and shock. In Experiment II, no evidence of response-shock association was obtained when open rearing was the punished response, and open rearing did not overshadow the tone. Punishment had some effect on face washing, but there was no statistically significant overshadowing with this response. These results are consistent with the idea that differences in punishment suppression among these three responses have an associative basis. They also demonstrate the usefulness of a novel paradigm for studying “preparedness” of response-reinforcer associations.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号