全文获取类型
收费全文 | 409篇 |
免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
412篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 6篇 |
2019年 | 7篇 |
2018年 | 4篇 |
2017年 | 7篇 |
2016年 | 6篇 |
2015年 | 3篇 |
2014年 | 5篇 |
2013年 | 42篇 |
2012年 | 11篇 |
2011年 | 9篇 |
2010年 | 8篇 |
2009年 | 11篇 |
2008年 | 17篇 |
2007年 | 13篇 |
2006年 | 11篇 |
2005年 | 18篇 |
2004年 | 7篇 |
2003年 | 19篇 |
2002年 | 13篇 |
2001年 | 3篇 |
1999年 | 7篇 |
1997年 | 3篇 |
1996年 | 4篇 |
1995年 | 7篇 |
1993年 | 9篇 |
1992年 | 8篇 |
1991年 | 6篇 |
1990年 | 5篇 |
1988年 | 5篇 |
1987年 | 6篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 7篇 |
1984年 | 7篇 |
1982年 | 7篇 |
1981年 | 5篇 |
1980年 | 10篇 |
1979年 | 4篇 |
1978年 | 8篇 |
1977年 | 5篇 |
1976年 | 12篇 |
1975年 | 5篇 |
1974年 | 6篇 |
1973年 | 12篇 |
1972年 | 5篇 |
1971年 | 6篇 |
1970年 | 5篇 |
1969年 | 2篇 |
1968年 | 2篇 |
1966年 | 4篇 |
1964年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有412条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
101.
Jerome M. Sattler 《Journal of School Psychology》1981,19(4):359-369
The issue of culturally biased intelligence tests has been heard in two federal courts. Judge Robert F. Peckham, in the Larry P., et al. v. Wilson Riles, et al. case, ruled that intelligence tests were culturally biased. In contrast, Judge John F. Grady, in the Parents in Action on Special Education, et al. v. Joseph P. Hannon, et al. case, ruled that intelligence tests were not culturally biased. A mock “interview” based on the judges' written opinions, shows similarities and differences in their approach to various issues involved in the trials. 相似文献
102.
Wachs TD Moussa W Bishry Z Yunis F Sobhy A Mccabe G Jerome N Galal O Harrison G Kirksey A 《Intelligence》1993,17(2):151-172
Two relatively unexplored questions--which specific components of chronic mild child malnutrition are associated with specific behavioral and cognitive deficits and do psychosocial child rearing factors moderate the nutrition-development relationship?--were investigated in a longitudinal study involving 153 toddlers from the Egyptian village of Kalama. Beginning at study entry at age 18 months and continuing until 30 months, food intake information was obtained for 2 consecutive days each month. During the same period, researchers visited the homes twice a month to observe both toddler behavior and caregiver styles. Finally, standardized infant development tests were administered at 24 months and 30 minutes and the capacity for exploratory, symbolic play was assessed. Specific components of nutritional intake at 18-23 months of age were confirmed to be uniquely related to specific developmental outcomes at 24 months of age. Changes in toddler intake from 18-30 months also predicted 30-month cognitive performance. At 24 months, general mental competence was associated positively with fat intake, total kilocalories, and total protein intake while symbolic play capacity was related only to total protein intake. Significant increases in predictive variance occurred when the model was expanded to encompass caregiver behaviors as well as nutritional factors. For example, when nonverbal response to child vocalizations was used as the environmental measure, there was evidence of both coactive and synergistic interactions for comparisons involving cognitive development indicators. For symbolic play, an increase in predictive variance was found when the model included the multiplicative interaction of animal kilocalories by nonverbal response to vocalizations. Also observed was a buffering effect against poor psychosocial rearing conditions given the presence of adequate nutritional intake. These findings suggest the feasibility of multidimensional risk profiles, the provision of specific nutrients targeted at specific deficits, and multidisciplinary interventions. 相似文献
103.
John J. DeFrancesco Jerome Taylor 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》1993,15(1):53-56
The revised (1987) Socialization Scale of the California Psychological Inventory was studied in a sample of 121 delinquent and nondelinquent males to determine if the revised scale, compared to the original version of the scale (1957), could differentiate among a group of nonoffenders and a comprehensive typology of offenders. Results of an ANOVA and product-moment correlations provided significant and considerable support for the discriminability of the revised So Scale. Obtained results indicate that the revised instrument can differentiate offenders from nonoffenders to about the same, or a greater, degree than the original scale. 相似文献
104.
W. Jay Dowling 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1991,50(4):305-313
In a continuous-running-memory task, subjects heard novel seven-note melodies that were tested after delays of 11 sec (empty) or 39 sec (filled). Test items were transposed to new pitch levels (to moderately distant keys in the musical sense)and included exact transpositions (targets), same-contour lures with altered pitch intervals, and new-contour lures. Melodies differed in tonal strength (degree of conformity to a musical key) and were tonally strong, tonally weak, or atonal. False alarms to same-contour lures decreased over the longer delay period, but only for tonal stimuli. In agreement with previous studies, discrimination of detailed changes in pitch intervals improved with increased delay, whereas discrimination of more global contour information declined, again only for tonal stimuli. These results suggest that poor short-delay performance in rejecting same-contour lures arises from confusion that is based on the similarity of tonality between standard stimuli and lures. If a test item has the same contour and a similar tonality to a just-presented item, subjects tend to accept it. After a delay filled with melodies in other tonalities, the salience of key information recedes, and subjects base their judgments on more detailed pattern information (namely, exact pitch intervals). The fact that tonality affects judgments of melodic contour indicates that contour is not an entirely separable feature of melodies but rather that a melody with its contour constitutes an integrated perceptual whole. 相似文献
105.
Jerome I. Gellman 《International Journal for Philosophy of Religion》2011,70(2):161-166
106.
This study compared differences in the Bender-Gestalt mean error scores of the 1,938 black, Hispanic, and white children in the System of Multicultural Pluralistic Assessment (SOMPA) standardization sample. A two-way between groups unweighted means ANOVA was performed on the error scores for the seven age groups and for the three ethnic groups. Significant differences were found for both main effects. Black children performed lower than (a) white children at every age level and (b) Hispanic children at all except one age level. Hispanic children performed similarly to white children at all except two age levels. Results support those reported by other investigations, but do not offer any clues to account for the findings. 相似文献
107.
108.
109.
110.