全文获取类型
收费全文 | 810篇 |
免费 | 24篇 |
专业分类
834篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 6篇 |
2023年 | 14篇 |
2022年 | 6篇 |
2021年 | 5篇 |
2020年 | 21篇 |
2019年 | 31篇 |
2018年 | 32篇 |
2017年 | 36篇 |
2016年 | 36篇 |
2015年 | 23篇 |
2014年 | 11篇 |
2013年 | 106篇 |
2012年 | 40篇 |
2011年 | 47篇 |
2010年 | 20篇 |
2009年 | 25篇 |
2008年 | 44篇 |
2007年 | 40篇 |
2006年 | 35篇 |
2005年 | 16篇 |
2004年 | 13篇 |
2003年 | 24篇 |
2002年 | 15篇 |
2001年 | 9篇 |
2000年 | 9篇 |
1999年 | 15篇 |
1998年 | 6篇 |
1997年 | 5篇 |
1995年 | 6篇 |
1994年 | 6篇 |
1992年 | 9篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1990年 | 6篇 |
1989年 | 7篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1986年 | 5篇 |
1984年 | 7篇 |
1982年 | 4篇 |
1980年 | 5篇 |
1978年 | 6篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
1976年 | 5篇 |
1975年 | 5篇 |
1974年 | 7篇 |
1973年 | 4篇 |
1970年 | 7篇 |
1969年 | 5篇 |
1968年 | 5篇 |
1967年 | 5篇 |
排序方式: 共有834条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
11.
Lana Bojanić Sandra Flynn Myrsini Gianatsi Navneet Kapur Louis Appleby Jenny Shaw 《Aggressive behavior》2020,46(6):516-522
Parricide is a rare type of homicide in which mental illness is often an important factor. The aims of this study were (a) to describe the characteristics of parricide offenders with a focus on mental illness and clinical care and (b) to examine Heide's widely used typology of parricide through a data-driven approach. We analyzed all homicides in England and Wales between 1997 and 2014. Parricide offenders in our sample were most often male, unmarried, and unemployed, with a third of offenders diagnosed with schizophrenia; 28% had been in contact with mental health services before the offense. The latent class analysis resulted in three types of parricide offenders: middle-aged with affective disorder, previously abused, and seriously mentally Ill, which confirmed, to an extent, Heide's typology. Health and social care services should actively engage with carers of people with mental illness and support to those caring for older relatives and victims of abuse. 相似文献
12.
Melissa Hamilton 《Behavioral sciences & the law》2020,38(3):226-245
Risk assessment tools driven by algorithms offer promising advantages in predicting the recidivism risk of defendants. Jurisdictions are increasingly relying upon risk tool outcomes to help judges at sentencing with their decisions on whether to incarcerate or whether to use community-based sanctions. Yet as sentencing has significant consequences for public safety and individual rights, care must be taken that the tools relied upon are appropriate for the task. Judges are encouraged to act as gatekeepers to evaluate whether the forensic risk assessment tool offered has a sufficient level of validity in that it is fit for the purposes of sentencing, provides an acceptable level of accuracy in its predictions, and achieves an adequate standard of reliability with regard to its outcomes. 相似文献
13.
Gaze following plays a role in parent–infant communication and is a key mechanism by which infants acquire information about the world from social input. Gaze following in Deaf infants has been understudied. Twelve Deaf infants of Deaf parents (DoD) who had native exposure to American Sign Language (ASL) were gender‐matched and age‐matched (±7 days) to 60 spoken‐language hearing control infants. Results showed that the DoD infants had significantly higher gaze‐following scores than the hearing infants. We hypothesize that in the absence of auditory input, and with support from ASL‐fluent Deaf parents, infants become attuned to visual‐communicative signals from other people, which engenders increased gaze following. These findings underscore the need to revise the ‘deficit model’ of deafness. Deaf infants immersed in natural sign language from birth are better at understanding the signals and identifying the referential meaning of adults’ gaze behavior compared to hearing infants not exposed to sign language. Broader implications for theories of social‐cognitive development are discussed. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at https://youtu.be/QXCDK_CUmAI 相似文献
14.
Viridiana L. Benitez Federica Bulgarelli Krista Byers‐Heinlein Jenny R. Saffran Daniel J. Weiss 《Developmental science》2020,23(2)
Language acquisition depends on the ability to detect and track the distributional properties of speech. Successful acquisition also necessitates detecting changes in those properties, which can occur when the learner encounters different speakers, topics, dialects, or languages. When encountering multiple speech streams with different underlying statistics but overlapping features, how do infants keep track of the properties of each speech stream separately? In four experiments, we tested whether 8‐month‐old monolingual infants (N = 144) can track the underlying statistics of two artificial speech streams that share a portion of their syllables. We first presented each stream individually. We then presented the two speech streams in sequence, without contextual cues signaling the different speech streams, and subsequently added pitch and accent cues to help learners track each stream separately. The results reveal that monolingual infants experience difficulty tracking the statistical regularities in two speech streams presented sequentially, even when provided with contextual cues intended to facilitate separation of the speech streams. We discuss the implications of our findings for understanding how infants learn and separate the input when confronted with multiple statistical structures. 相似文献
15.
16.
17.
18.
Previous research has demonstrated that it is possible to induce biases in the interpretation of ambiguous text passages by training. Participants consistently trained to interpret emotionally ambiguous passages in either a negative or positive direction show training-congruent effects when presented with new ambiguous material. These training effects are demonstrated by participants' subsequent recognition ratings for disambiguating sentences, which represent both possible meanings of the novel ambiguous test passages. In this series of experiments, we investigated the durability of these training effects over time spans of up to 1 day and found them to be robust. The findings encourage us to believe that induced biases may serve as a useful analogue to those observed clinically. 相似文献
19.
Klatzky RL Lederman SJ Hamilton C Grindley M Swendsen RH 《Perception & psychophysics》2003,65(4):613-631
Vibratory roughness perception occurs when people feel a surface with a rigid probe. Accordingly, perceived roughness should reflect probe and surface geometry, exploratory speed, and force. Experiments 1 and 2 compared magnitude estimation of roughness with the bare finger and two types of probes, one designed to eliminate force moments, under the subject's active control. Experiments 3 and 4 varied speed under passive control. Log magnitude was consistently a quadratic function of log spacing between elements in the surface. The location of the function's peak was related to the drop point--that is, the spacing at which the probe can just drop between elements--which is affected by probe tip diameter, element height, and speed. Other parameters of the quadratic were affected by probe type and speed. 相似文献
20.