全文获取类型
收费全文 | 5293篇 |
免费 | 67篇 |
专业分类
5360篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 29篇 |
2023年 | 71篇 |
2022年 | 35篇 |
2021年 | 68篇 |
2020年 | 162篇 |
2019年 | 124篇 |
2018年 | 180篇 |
2017年 | 244篇 |
2016年 | 242篇 |
2015年 | 181篇 |
2014年 | 201篇 |
2013年 | 720篇 |
2012年 | 288篇 |
2011年 | 267篇 |
2010年 | 170篇 |
2009年 | 212篇 |
2008年 | 255篇 |
2007年 | 238篇 |
2006年 | 191篇 |
2005年 | 153篇 |
2004年 | 155篇 |
2003年 | 162篇 |
2002年 | 136篇 |
2001年 | 54篇 |
2000年 | 50篇 |
1999年 | 52篇 |
1998年 | 57篇 |
1997年 | 43篇 |
1996年 | 71篇 |
1995年 | 64篇 |
1994年 | 41篇 |
1993年 | 50篇 |
1992年 | 40篇 |
1991年 | 42篇 |
1990年 | 38篇 |
1989年 | 25篇 |
1988年 | 19篇 |
1987年 | 20篇 |
1986年 | 20篇 |
1985年 | 10篇 |
1984年 | 14篇 |
1983年 | 9篇 |
1982年 | 11篇 |
1981年 | 13篇 |
1980年 | 17篇 |
1979年 | 21篇 |
1978年 | 23篇 |
1977年 | 24篇 |
1974年 | 6篇 |
1971年 | 7篇 |
排序方式: 共有5360条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
McCabe J Hartman M 《Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition》2008,15(5):574-600
Two experiments examined age differences in mechanisms hypothesized to affect activation of item and temporal information in working memory. Activation levels were inferred from the ability to reject n-back lures matching items in different temporal positions. Information with the least decay had a performance advantage over less recent information, but was susceptible to the same temporal context errors found in all adjacent-to-target lure positions. Lures most distant from the current item showed a performance rebound. The pattern of increased magnitudes of age effects at adjacent-to-target positions indicated a reduction in older adults' working memory for temporal context information above and beyond item memory declines. Results overall support the emphasis on context information as a critical factor in working memory and cognitive aging. 相似文献
992.
993.
994.
Norman Y. Teng PhD 《亚洲哲学》2006,16(1):39-47
The anecdote of Zhuangzi and Hui Shi's brief discussion on a bridge above the Hao river gives us a nice piece of reasoning in ancient Chinese texts that may serve as a platform for a productive philosophical exchange between the East and the West. The present study examines Hansen's inferential analysis of Zhuangzi and Hui Shi's discussion in this spirit. It is argued that Hansen's analysis founders. To do justice to both Hui Shi and Zhuangzi, the present study proposes that we apply the logic developed in the later Mohist text, the Lesser Pick, to an analysis of their discussion. The re-analysis shows that the intricate dialectic of the reasoning in which Zhuangzi and Hui Shi engage, neatly accords with the pattern of discourse expounded in the Lesser Pick, and gives us global insight into Zhuangzi's final statement in the anecdote, which is notoriously recondite, or confusing. 相似文献
995.
Jennifer L. Ramsey-Rennels Judith H. Langlois 《Current directions in psychological science》2006,15(2):59-62
ABSTRACT— Infants show an interesting asymmetry in face processing: They are more fluent in processing female faces than they are at processing male faces. We hypothesize that such processing asymmetry results from greater experience with female faces than with male faces early in development. Asymmetrical face processing may have long-lasting implications for development of face recognition, development of knowledge structures regarding females and males, and social-information processing. We encourage researchers to use both female and male faces in their face-perception research and to conduct separate analyses for female and male faces. 相似文献
996.
Ego depletion by response exaggeration 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Brandon J. Schmeichel Heath A. Demaree Jennifer L. Robinson 《Journal of experimental social psychology》2006,42(1):95-102
Suppressing or inhibiting responses has a host of negative effects, including a temporary reduction in self-regulatory strength (ego depletion). Less attention has been given to response exaggeration, which should also deplete regulatory strength and therefore disrupt subsequent self-control. We tested the depletion hypothesis by having participants perform tests of cognitive fluency after exaggerating responses (or not) to a disgusting film clip. Response exaggeration produced increased emotional expression but did not increase subjective emotional experience. Moreover, exaggerating disgust reactions impaired subsequent performance on tests of cognitive fluency. The cognitive aftereffects of exaggeration were not attributable to emotional experience or to changes in sympathetic or parasympathetic arousal (as indicated by skin conductance and heart rate variability high frequency power, respectively). Poorer cognitive fluency after response exaggeration indicates a detrimental effect of purposeful self-regulation. 相似文献
997.
Arnold JE Kam CL Tanenhaus MK 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2007,33(5):914-930
Eye-tracking and gating experiments examined reference comprehension with fluent (Click on the red. . .) and disfluent (Click on [pause] thee uh red . . .) instructions while listeners viewed displays with 2 familiar (e.g., ice cream cones) and 2 unfamiliar objects (e.g., squiggly shapes). Disfluent instructions made unfamiliar objects more expected, which influenced listeners' on-line hypotheses from the onset of the color word. The unfamiliarity bias was sharply reduced by instructions that the speaker had object agnosia, and thus difficulty naming familiar objects (Experiment 2), but was not affected by intermittent sources of speaker distraction (beeps and construction noises; Experiments 3). The authors conclude that listeners can make situation-specific inferences about likely sources of disfluency, but there are some limitations to these attributions. 相似文献
998.
Kleinman JT Newhart M Davis C Heidler-Gary J Gottesman RF Hillis AE 《Brain and cognition》2007,64(1):50-59
The frequency of various types of unilateral spatial neglect and associated areas of neural dysfunction after left hemisphere stroke are not well characterized. Unilateral spatial neglect (USN) in distinct spatial reference frames have been identified after acute right, but not left hemisphere stroke. We studied 47 consecutive right handed patients within 48h of left hemisphere stroke to determine the frequency and distribution of types of right USN using cognitive testing and MRI imaging. The distribution of USN types was different from the previously reported distribution following acute right hemisphere stroke. In this left hemisphere stroke population, allocentric neglect was more frequent than egocentric neglect. 相似文献
999.
Wilkins AJ Smith J Willison CK Beare T Boyd A Hardy G Mell L Peach C Harper S 《Perception》2007,36(12):1788-1803
In a series of eight studies it is shown that the first peak in the horizontal autocorrelation of the image of a word (which captures the similarity in shape between the neighbouring strokes of letters) determines (i) the appearance of the words as striped; (ii) the speed with which the words are read, both aloud and silently; and (iii) the speed with which a paragraph of text can be searched. By subtly distorting the horizontal dimension of text, and thereby reducing the first peak in the horizontal autocorrelation, it is shown that the speed of word recognition can be increased. The increase in speed is greater in poor readers. 相似文献
1000.
The effects of pregnancy on hiring decisions during employment interviews are examined in a United States sample of 210 undergraduate
business school students at a Midwestern university. A pregnant applicant was compared to a non-pregnant applicant with identical
credentials and interview performance to explore any differences in interviewer ratings of qualifications and hiring by having
participants view videotaped interviews. Results show that in spite of being viewed as equally qualified and well-suited for
the job, the pregnant applicant received significantly lower hiring recommendation ratings. The pregnant applicant was also
rated as more likely to need time off, miss work and quit compared to the non-pregnant applicant, indicating a concern about
absenteeism regarding the pregnant applicant. 相似文献