全文获取类型
收费全文 | 5097篇 |
免费 | 69篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
5168篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 30篇 |
2023年 | 41篇 |
2022年 | 33篇 |
2021年 | 76篇 |
2020年 | 102篇 |
2019年 | 137篇 |
2018年 | 195篇 |
2017年 | 210篇 |
2016年 | 205篇 |
2015年 | 137篇 |
2014年 | 163篇 |
2013年 | 548篇 |
2012年 | 314篇 |
2011年 | 325篇 |
2010年 | 166篇 |
2009年 | 191篇 |
2008年 | 292篇 |
2007年 | 301篇 |
2006年 | 227篇 |
2005年 | 192篇 |
2004年 | 208篇 |
2003年 | 208篇 |
2002年 | 175篇 |
2001年 | 75篇 |
2000年 | 60篇 |
1999年 | 55篇 |
1998年 | 64篇 |
1997年 | 55篇 |
1996年 | 49篇 |
1995年 | 45篇 |
1994年 | 34篇 |
1993年 | 34篇 |
1992年 | 20篇 |
1991年 | 19篇 |
1990年 | 18篇 |
1989年 | 12篇 |
1988年 | 12篇 |
1987年 | 16篇 |
1986年 | 7篇 |
1985年 | 16篇 |
1984年 | 12篇 |
1983年 | 13篇 |
1982年 | 7篇 |
1981年 | 14篇 |
1980年 | 7篇 |
1979年 | 11篇 |
1978年 | 4篇 |
1977年 | 5篇 |
1976年 | 5篇 |
1974年 | 7篇 |
排序方式: 共有5168条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
61.
62.
63.
Summary In this study the role of perceptual and motor factors on the motor organization (integrated versus parallel) adopted by musically skilled and unskilled subjects in a polyrhythmic tapping task was investigated. Subjects tapped a 3:2 polyrhythm to match the timing of two isochronous tone trains, one tone train for each hand. Perceptual factors were examined by the manipulation of the frequency difference between the tone trains to produce either an integrated or a streamed percept. Motor factors were examined by comparison of performance on two versions of the 3:2 polyrhythm. In one (simultaneous) version, each cycle of the polyrhythm began with a simultaneous left- and right-hand tap. In the other (shifted) version a 100-ms interval was introduced between the initial left and right taps in each cycle. Examination of the pattern of variances and covariances among intertap intervals suggested that most of the subjects in this study adopted an integrated motor organization that involved interleaving the timing of the two hands. Further analysis revealed that a serial chained model described the pattern of covariances best for the simultaneous pattern, whereas a hierarchical organization described the pattern of covariances for the shifted pattern best. The finding that performance was more accurate with integrated tones than with streamed tones provides some support for a perceptual-motor facilitation hypothesis. 相似文献
64.
Obbie Tyler Todd 《International Journal of Systematic Theology》2019,21(3):251-264
The American moral governmental theory of the atonement was distinct from other Protestant views in several ways, but truly unique in one. Its definition of forgiveness as a pardon instead of a debt paid, its insistence that Christ died for the good of the moral universe, and its rejection of the distributive understanding of penal substitution all stemmed from its relentless commitment to view both God and humanity in terms of public life. It is the purpose of this article to demonstrate that the most foundational element in the American moral governmental theory of the atonement was the idea of publicity, namely the public nature it ascribed to sin, salvation and the glory of God. 相似文献
65.
Jennifer A. Herdt 《The Journal of religious ethics》2019,47(1):68-93
Eudaimonism is often regarded as egoistic. If it recommends that agents pursue their own good because it is their own good, it is guilty as charged. But excellence‐prior eudaimonism offers a non‐egoistic alternative to this welfare‐prior eudaimonism. Excellence‐prior eudaimonism recommends that an agent live in a way that is in fact good for the agent, but it does not regard the agent’s own good as necessarily that for the sake of which the agent acts, nor does it regard living well as justified by the fact that it is good for the agent, but simply because it is good. The Christian eudaimonisms of Augustine and Aquinas are best understood as deepened forms of excellence‐prior eudaimonism. 相似文献
66.
In experiment 1, pigeons were trained to match temporal (2, 8, and 10 s of houselight) and location (feeder light, left key,
center key illumination) samples to color comparison stimuli. Red choices were correct following the 2-s and feeder light,
orange choices were correct following the 8-s and center key, and green choices were correct following the 10-s and left key.
Samples that were harder to discriminate (8- vs 10-s, and left vs center key) were mapped onto comparisons that were easy
to discriminate (orange vs green), while samples that were easier to discriminate (2- vs 8-s, and feeder light vs left key)
were mapped onto comparisons that were hard to discriminate(red vs orange). The pattern of errors for temporal and location
samples indicated that these samples were not represented by a common code even though they were associated with the same
comparison stimuli. In experiment 2, the same pigeons were trained with visual samples in which samples that were hard to
discriminate (triangle vs circle) were mapped onto comparisons that were easy to discriminate (orange vs green), while samples
that were easy to discriminate(plus vs triangle) were mapped onto comparisons that were hard to discriminate (red vs orange).
Following acquisition of the visual discrimination, the temporal samples were re-introduced and many-to-one training was continued.
During delay testing, the pattern of errors for temporal and visual samples was equivalent and consistent with the hypothesis
that visual samples were being coded in terms of the duration appropriate for the temporal sample with which it shared a common
comparison response. Data from no-sample test sessions ruled out a simple response bias explanation of the data. The properties
of common codes for temporal and nontemporal events can be somewhat flexible and more complicated than previously envisaged.
Received: 21 January 1998 / Accepted after revision: 5 April 1998 相似文献
67.
The American Journal of Psychoanalysis - 相似文献
68.
Marilyn Lewis Lanza Jennifer Anderson Helene Satz James Stone Herbert L. Kayne Kathleen Smith Kathleen Rideout 《Group》1998,22(1):15-37
An observational scale to record aggressive behaviors, defenses, and interventions that occur during a psychotherapy group has been developed. Content and process items were included. The content items focus on the spoken material and are on a continuum of increasing group members' awareness of and responsibility for their anger. The process items are directed to underlying psychodynamic issues on a continuum from avoiding anger, to indirect and then expression of anger. Leader interventions (responses) are also included. The development of the scale, the Aggression Observation Scale for Group Psychotherapy (AOSGP), is described including pilot testing and reliability and validity findings. 相似文献
69.
Jennifer Dorfman 《Memory & cognition》1998,26(6):1157-1172
This study provides further evidence for the notion, proposed by Dorfman (1994), that activation of sublexical components plays an important role in implicit memory for novel words. Priming in an identification task was observed consistently when nonwords were formed out of morphemes; weaker, more variable effects were observed when nonwords were composed of syllables or pseudosyllables. Priming was insensitive to changes in the surface features (type case) of the stimuli and was disrupted by rearrangement of the stimulus components. Results are interpreted in terms of the activation and integration of preexisting sublexical representations and as evidence against the role of newly established episodic or perceptual representations. It is concluded that findings of priming for novel information should not be taken as evidence against an activation view. 相似文献
70.