全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4010篇 |
免费 | 51篇 |
专业分类
4061篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 27篇 |
2023年 | 34篇 |
2022年 | 33篇 |
2021年 | 65篇 |
2020年 | 83篇 |
2019年 | 115篇 |
2018年 | 170篇 |
2017年 | 181篇 |
2016年 | 171篇 |
2015年 | 118篇 |
2014年 | 136篇 |
2013年 | 435篇 |
2012年 | 256篇 |
2011年 | 255篇 |
2010年 | 132篇 |
2009年 | 159篇 |
2008年 | 234篇 |
2007年 | 228篇 |
2006年 | 172篇 |
2005年 | 151篇 |
2004年 | 155篇 |
2003年 | 156篇 |
2002年 | 128篇 |
2001年 | 47篇 |
2000年 | 46篇 |
1999年 | 41篇 |
1998年 | 50篇 |
1997年 | 39篇 |
1996年 | 35篇 |
1995年 | 31篇 |
1994年 | 21篇 |
1993年 | 26篇 |
1992年 | 11篇 |
1991年 | 9篇 |
1990年 | 8篇 |
1989年 | 12篇 |
1988年 | 5篇 |
1987年 | 10篇 |
1986年 | 7篇 |
1985年 | 10篇 |
1984年 | 9篇 |
1983年 | 7篇 |
1982年 | 4篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 5篇 |
1978年 | 5篇 |
1976年 | 3篇 |
1971年 | 3篇 |
1963年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有4061条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
221.
The emotional Stroop task provides an experimental measure of selective attention to emotional information. In the current study, the emotional Stroop effect was examined in a Spanish–English bilingual speaking population. The results revealed that the emotional Stroop effect is a robust phenomenon and replicable within a bilingual population. Furthermore, highly proficient bilinguals demonstrated equal interference effects in both their first language and their second language. The current study provides evidence of the automatic activation of emotional components in words appearing in more than one language. These results are discussed with reference to previous findings in the literature on bilingual emotion word representation. 相似文献
222.
223.
Christi A. Patten Colin A. Armstorng John E. Martin James F. Sallis Jennifer Booth 《Psychology & health》2013,28(4):571-581
Abstract Building on our previous six study series, the current studies 7 and 8 evaluated behavioral methods of improving adherence to exercise in adults. The studies tested the effectiveness of a single exercise modality versus choice of a variety of exercises and were conducted as part of a 12-week extended studies college course in San Diego, CA. Recruitment efforts were targeted to individuals who reported that they were sedentary and/or had experienced difficulty adhering to regular exercise. Participants were 42 adults (32F, 10M; mean age = 42.0) in Study 7 and 48 adults (35F, 8M; mean age = 47.9) in Study 8. In each study, subjects were randomly assigned to a single focus or variety exercise condition. There were no significant group differences in class attendance or out-of-class exercise adherence rates in Studies 7 or 8. In Study 7, significantly lower drop-out rates were detected in the single focus group (19%) than the variety condition (47.6%) (p = 0.04). No significant group differences were detected for drop-out rates in Study 8. Suggestions for further research are offered. 相似文献
224.
225.
Jennifer M. FletcherAnthony D.G. Marks Donald W. Hine 《Journal of research in personality》2012,46(1):40-48
This study of adults (n = 269) and adolescents (n = 308) applied latent profile analysis to preference for rational and/or experiential cognition, coupled with working memory capacity (WMC). A 4-profile solution comprising rationally dominant, experientially dominant, dual preference, and disengaged groups was retained for both adult and adolescent samples. Our solution indicated that high WMC was associated with both preference and ability to engage in rational processing. Profile membership significantly discriminated both adults and adolescents on several reasoning tasks and measures of cognitive biases. Overall our results indicate that cognitive processing styles and WMC can be combined to create a typology that distinguishes between four types of thinkers who significantly differ in their performance on reasoning tasks. 相似文献
226.
It was tested whether boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), subgrouped by aggressive status, would show higher rates of depressive symptomatology and lower levels of self-esteem than would comparison boys and, in a subsample, explored attributional mechanisms that may be related to such internalizing features. Study 1 utilized 114 boys with ADHD (all prior recipients of stimulant medication) and 87 comparison boys, aged 7–12 years. Aggressive boys with ADHD reported more symptoms of depression than did nonaggressive boys with ADHD, who, in turn, reported more depression than did comparison boys. Effect sizes were moderate to large and did not vary with a depression rating scale uncontaminated by ADHD-related items. For self-esteem, the most pronounced effect was that aggressive boys with ADHD showed lower levels than did nonaggressive ADHD or comparison boys; effects were again moderate to large. Study 2 participants were a subsample of boys with ADHD from Study 1 (N = 27). We probed causal attributions in ADHD-related domains through responses to hypothetical vignettes, in which the protagonist's medication status (medicated, not medicated) was crossed with type of outcome (good, bad). Medication-related attributions were frequent. In describing the protagonist's success in relation to medication treatment, the sample showed significant associations between (a) medication-related attributions and (b) increased depressive symptomatology as well as decreased self-esteem. We discuss attributional processes that may help to explain the variation in internalizing symptoms among children with ADHD. 相似文献
227.
228.
Jennifer J. Muehlenkamp Margaret L. Cowles Peter M. Gutierrez 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2010,32(2):236-245
Suicide and non-suicidal self-injurious behavior (NSSI) continue to be significant health problems for adolescents. Although
there are many instruments available to assess self-harm behaviors, few have been validated for use with adolescents, and
even fewer have been validated for use with different racial/ethnic groups. The Self-Harm Behavior Questionnaire (SHBQ) assesses
NSSI, suicidal ideation, suicide threats, and past suicide attempts. Data from 1,386 racially/ethnically diverse adolescents
(57.5% minority) were collected to examine the validity of the SHBQ in this age group. Analyses supported the four-factor
structure, revealed the SHBQ is internally consistent, and provided evidence for convergent validity within Caucasian, African
American, and Hispanic adolescents. However, slight differences were noted. Overall, the findings suggest the SHBQ is a useful
assessment tool appropriate for use with Caucasian, African American, and Hispanic adolescents. 相似文献
229.
Church BA Krauss MS Lopata C Toomey JA Thomeer ML Coutinho MV Volker MA Mercado E 《Psychonomic bulletin & review》2010,17(6):862-868
Children with autism spectrum disorder process many perceptual and social events differently from typically developing children,
suggesting that they may also form and recognize categories differently. We used a dot pattern categorization task and prototype
comparison modeling to compare categorical processing in children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder and matched
typical controls. We were interested in whether there were differences in how children with autism use average similarity
information about a category to make decisions. During testing, the group with autism spectrum disorder endorsed prototypes
less and was seemingly less sensitive to differences between to-be-categorized items and the prototype. The findings suggest
that individuals with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder are less likely to use overall average similarity when forming
categories or making categorical decisions. Such differences in category formation and use may negatively impact processing
of socially relevant information, such as facial expressions. A supplemental appendix for this article may be downloaded from
http://pbr.psychonomic-journals.org/content/supplemental. 相似文献
230.
Mark S. Edwards Jennifer S. Burt Ottmar V. Lipp 《British journal of psychology (London, England : 1953)》2010,101(2):325-343
We investigated selective attention for masked and unmasked, threat, and positively valenced words, in high trait anxious (HTA) and low trait anxious (LTA) individuals using the emotional Stroop colour‐naming task. State anxiety was varied within participants through the threat of electric shock. To investigate whether the sequencing of the state anxiety manipulation affected colour‐naming latencies, the ordering of the shock threat and shock safe conditions was counterbalanced across participants. The results indicated that the ordering of the state anxiety manipulation moderated masked and unmasked threat bias effects. Specifically, relative to LTA individuals, HTA individuals showed a threat interference effect, but this effect was limited to those who performed under the threat of shock in the later stages of the experiment. Irrespective of exposure mode and state anxiety status, all individuals showed interference for threat in the early stages of the experiment, relative to a threat facilitation effect in the later stages of the experiment. For the unmasked trials alone, the data also revealed a significant threat interference effect for the HTA group relative to the LTA group in the shock threat condition, and this effect was evident irrespective of shock threat order. The results are discussed with respect to the automatic nature of emotional processing in anxiety. 相似文献