全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3982篇 |
免费 | 53篇 |
专业分类
4035篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 27篇 |
2023年 | 34篇 |
2022年 | 33篇 |
2021年 | 65篇 |
2020年 | 79篇 |
2019年 | 115篇 |
2018年 | 173篇 |
2017年 | 183篇 |
2016年 | 171篇 |
2015年 | 118篇 |
2014年 | 139篇 |
2013年 | 440篇 |
2012年 | 256篇 |
2011年 | 257篇 |
2010年 | 131篇 |
2009年 | 156篇 |
2008年 | 236篇 |
2007年 | 227篇 |
2006年 | 172篇 |
2005年 | 150篇 |
2004年 | 153篇 |
2003年 | 154篇 |
2002年 | 129篇 |
2001年 | 45篇 |
2000年 | 43篇 |
1999年 | 40篇 |
1998年 | 49篇 |
1997年 | 39篇 |
1996年 | 35篇 |
1995年 | 31篇 |
1994年 | 21篇 |
1993年 | 26篇 |
1992年 | 10篇 |
1991年 | 8篇 |
1990年 | 7篇 |
1989年 | 9篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 9篇 |
1986年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 8篇 |
1984年 | 7篇 |
1983年 | 8篇 |
1982年 | 4篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 5篇 |
1978年 | 4篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1974年 | 3篇 |
1971年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有4035条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
931.
Mary C. Murphy Jennifer A. Richeson Daniel C. Molden 《Social and Personality Psychology Compass》2011,5(2):118-131
As the United States becomes increasingly diverse, interracial contact will become considerably less rare. Much research has suggested that interracial interactions are often stressful and uncomfortable for both Whites and racial minorities. Bringing together several bodies of research, the present article outlines a motivational perspective on the dynamics of intergroup contact. To this end, we consider the roles of three motivational mindsets that have the potential to shape interactions to be less cognitively depleting and more enjoyable for both interactants. In particular, we consider the effects of (i) approach and avoidance motivation (Handbook of Motivation and Cognition: Foundation of Social Behavior, 1990, New York: Guilford Press), (ii) promotion and prevention regulatory focus (American Psychologist, 52 , 1997, 1280), and (iii) learning and performance goals (Psychological Review, 95 , 1988, 256) in shaping the dynamics of interracial contact. We suggest that investigations into these motivational mindsets will offer further insight into how and why interracial interactions go awry and will assist in the development of strategies and interventions that facilitate more smooth and enjoyable contact experiences. 相似文献
932.
933.
A heterogeneous sample of adult women and men responded to a Photo Choice Task (PCT) in which they were asked to make 24 choices between two persons for a hypothetical interaction; eight of the choices were between a woman and a man. Participants also responded to a measure of adherence to Sex Role Stereotypes and to a measure of Adversarial Sexual Beliefs. On the eight choices between a woman and a man on the PCT, men over 30 and younger than 30 chose women below chance level. Women over 30 made other-gender and same-gender choices that did not differ from chance, but the women under 30 chose women over men reliably more than chance. Small but significant positive correlations were found among the scores on the PCT and the belief measures; for the total sample, the greater the choice of men over women for hypothetical interactions, the greater the agreement with sex role stereotypes and with adversarial sexual beliefs. An examination of mean differences in belief scores among groups of persons whose PCT scores (choice of men over women) were low, medium, and high revealed that men who were more likely to turn away from women in hypothetical situations were also more likely to adhere to stereotyped beliefs about sex roles and to view relationships between women and men as adversarial. This same pattern of individual differences was found among women but, for women, only the relationship between PCT scores and sex role stereotyping was significant. 相似文献
934.
Berdahl JL 《The Journal of applied psychology》2007,92(2):425-437
In 3 studies, the author tested 2 competing views of sexual harassment: (a) It is motivated primarily by sexual desire and, therefore, is directed at women who meet feminine ideals, and (b) it is motivated primarily by a desire to punish gender-role deviants and, therefore, is directed at women who violate feminine ideals. Study 1 included male and female college students (N = 175) and showed that women with relatively masculine personalities (e.g., assertive, dominant, and independent) experienced the most sexual harassment. Study 2 (N = 134) showed that this effect was not because women with relatively masculine personalities were more likely than others to negatively evaluate potentially harassing scenarios. Study 3 included male and female employees at 5 organizations (N = 238) and showed that women in male-dominated organizations were harassed more than women in female-dominated organizations, and that women in male-dominated organizations who had relatively masculine personalities were sexually harassed the most. 相似文献
935.
James M. Olson Neal J. Roesesc Jennifer Meen D. Joyce Robertson 《Journal of applied social psychology》1995,25(11):944-964
Two field studies assessed the preconditions and behavioral consequences of relative deprivation. A sample of single mothers receiving government assistance participated in Study 1, and a sample of working women participated in Study 2. We found general support for a 2-factor model of the preconditions of relative deprivation, in which wanting the desired state and deserving the desired state were the best predictors of discontent. In both studies, discontent regarding the status of one's group (i.e., single mothers or working women) was specifically predictive of reports of group-directed protest behaviors. In Study 2, discontent regarding one's own status was specifically predictive of willingness to engage in self-improvement behaviors. Participants in both studies reported more group discontent than personal discontent. These findings provide support from naturalistic settings for the construct of relative deprivation. 相似文献
936.
937.
Without visual attention, even the obvious–like a gorilla walking through a scene of people–goes undetected (Mack & Rock, 1998; Simons & Chabris, 1999). This “inattentional blindness” is a persistent, well-documented limitation of the human visual system. The current research examines whether social meaning reduces this visual bias by imbuing unexpected objects with signal value, thus increasing their relevance and facilitating perception. Using one of the most established illustrations of inattentional blindness, we show for the first time that activating a social association, even an erroneous one (i.e., the African American–ape association), drastically attenuates inattentional blindness. This is not accounted for by visual feature matching. Rather, these results suggest that social meaning, even when flawed, may direct our visual system towards associated visual information that would otherwise be overlooked. As such, these results provide a powerful replication of the African American–ape association and illustrate that this broadly held association has the power to spontaneously change the content of one's visual world. 相似文献
938.
Retrieving information from memory makes that information more recallable in the future than it otherwise would have been.
Optimizing retrieval practice has been assumed, on the basis of evidence and arguments tracing back to Landauer and Bjork
(1978), to require an expanding-interval schedule of successive retrievals, but recent findings suggest that expanding retrieval
practice may be inferior to uniform-interval retrieval practice when memory is tested after a long retention interval. We
report three experiments in which participants read educational passages and were then repeatedly tested, without feedback,
after an expanding or uniform sequence of intervals. On a test 1 week later, recall was enhanced by the expanding schedule,
but only when the task between successive retrievals was highly interfering with memory for the passage. These results suggest
that the extent to which learners benefit from expanding retrieval practice depends on the degree to which the to-belearned
information is vulnerable to forgetting. 相似文献
939.
Two experiments examined how observers' ability to perceive biological motion changes with increasing age. The observers discriminated among kinetic figures, depicting walking, jogging, and skipping. The direction, duration, and temporal correspondence of the motions were manipulated. Quantitative differences occurred between the recognition performances of younger and older observers, but these differences were often modest. The older and younger observers' performances were comparable for most conditions at stimulus durations of 400 ms. The older observers also performed well above chance at shorter durations of 240 and 120 ms. Unlike their performance on other 2- or 3-dimensional motion tasks, older observers' ability to perceive biological motion is relatively well preserved. 相似文献
940.
James Juergensen Joseph S. Weaver Kevin J. Burns Peter E. Knutson Jennifer L. Butler Heath A. Demaree 《Motivation and emotion》2014,38(2):297-304
The present research was designed to better understand how the magnitude of experienced surprise is affected by both individual difference variables as well as variations in surprise-eliciting stimuli. Eighty-five participants played 5 versions of a slot machine-like game. The five games only differed with respect to the probability of winning each trial—10 % (i.e., wins were highly unusual), 30, 50, 70, and 90 % (i.e., losses were highly unusual). Players were given a fictitious “bankroll” at the beginning of each game and played up to 25 trials of each game. On each trial, players selected their wager amount, “pulled” the handle, learned the outcome (win or loss), and reported their surprise level using a 1 (none) to 9 (extremely) Likert scale. Replicating past research, results revealed that self-reported surprise was inversely related to outcome probability and that wins were rated as more surprising than losses, even when wins and losses occurred at the same level of probability. Novel results include finding that larger wagers predicted greater felt surprise (regardless of outcome), and that women reported greater surprise to both wins and losses than men. 相似文献