全文获取类型
收费全文 | 5415篇 |
免费 | 79篇 |
专业分类
5494篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 45篇 |
2023年 | 54篇 |
2022年 | 45篇 |
2021年 | 93篇 |
2020年 | 125篇 |
2019年 | 180篇 |
2018年 | 245篇 |
2017年 | 242篇 |
2016年 | 245篇 |
2015年 | 161篇 |
2014年 | 199篇 |
2013年 | 617篇 |
2012年 | 341篇 |
2011年 | 355篇 |
2010年 | 184篇 |
2009年 | 200篇 |
2008年 | 287篇 |
2007年 | 279篇 |
2006年 | 223篇 |
2005年 | 187篇 |
2004年 | 191篇 |
2003年 | 187篇 |
2002年 | 153篇 |
2001年 | 59篇 |
2000年 | 72篇 |
1999年 | 58篇 |
1998年 | 61篇 |
1997年 | 53篇 |
1996年 | 43篇 |
1995年 | 36篇 |
1994年 | 26篇 |
1993年 | 31篇 |
1992年 | 20篇 |
1991年 | 18篇 |
1990年 | 11篇 |
1989年 | 13篇 |
1988年 | 7篇 |
1987年 | 15篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 19篇 |
1984年 | 16篇 |
1983年 | 14篇 |
1982年 | 7篇 |
1981年 | 8篇 |
1980年 | 8篇 |
1979年 | 14篇 |
1978年 | 6篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
1976年 | 6篇 |
1975年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有5494条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
301.
Jennifer J. Muehlenkamp Margaret L. Cowles Peter M. Gutierrez 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2010,32(2):236-245
Suicide and non-suicidal self-injurious behavior (NSSI) continue to be significant health problems for adolescents. Although
there are many instruments available to assess self-harm behaviors, few have been validated for use with adolescents, and
even fewer have been validated for use with different racial/ethnic groups. The Self-Harm Behavior Questionnaire (SHBQ) assesses
NSSI, suicidal ideation, suicide threats, and past suicide attempts. Data from 1,386 racially/ethnically diverse adolescents
(57.5% minority) were collected to examine the validity of the SHBQ in this age group. Analyses supported the four-factor
structure, revealed the SHBQ is internally consistent, and provided evidence for convergent validity within Caucasian, African
American, and Hispanic adolescents. However, slight differences were noted. Overall, the findings suggest the SHBQ is a useful
assessment tool appropriate for use with Caucasian, African American, and Hispanic adolescents. 相似文献
302.
Church BA Krauss MS Lopata C Toomey JA Thomeer ML Coutinho MV Volker MA Mercado E 《Psychonomic bulletin & review》2010,17(6):862-868
Children with autism spectrum disorder process many perceptual and social events differently from typically developing children,
suggesting that they may also form and recognize categories differently. We used a dot pattern categorization task and prototype
comparison modeling to compare categorical processing in children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder and matched
typical controls. We were interested in whether there were differences in how children with autism use average similarity
information about a category to make decisions. During testing, the group with autism spectrum disorder endorsed prototypes
less and was seemingly less sensitive to differences between to-be-categorized items and the prototype. The findings suggest
that individuals with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder are less likely to use overall average similarity when forming
categories or making categorical decisions. Such differences in category formation and use may negatively impact processing
of socially relevant information, such as facial expressions. A supplemental appendix for this article may be downloaded from
http://pbr.psychonomic-journals.org/content/supplemental. 相似文献
303.
Mark S. Edwards Jennifer S. Burt Ottmar V. Lipp 《British journal of psychology (London, England : 1953)》2010,101(2):325-343
We investigated selective attention for masked and unmasked, threat, and positively valenced words, in high trait anxious (HTA) and low trait anxious (LTA) individuals using the emotional Stroop colour‐naming task. State anxiety was varied within participants through the threat of electric shock. To investigate whether the sequencing of the state anxiety manipulation affected colour‐naming latencies, the ordering of the shock threat and shock safe conditions was counterbalanced across participants. The results indicated that the ordering of the state anxiety manipulation moderated masked and unmasked threat bias effects. Specifically, relative to LTA individuals, HTA individuals showed a threat interference effect, but this effect was limited to those who performed under the threat of shock in the later stages of the experiment. Irrespective of exposure mode and state anxiety status, all individuals showed interference for threat in the early stages of the experiment, relative to a threat facilitation effect in the later stages of the experiment. For the unmasked trials alone, the data also revealed a significant threat interference effect for the HTA group relative to the LTA group in the shock threat condition, and this effect was evident irrespective of shock threat order. The results are discussed with respect to the automatic nature of emotional processing in anxiety. 相似文献
304.
Scott R. Hinze Daniel G. Slaten William S. Horton Ryan Jenkins David N. Rapp 《Memory & cognition》2014,42(2):305-324
People rely on information they read even when it is inaccurate (Marsh, Meade, & Roediger, Journal of Memory and Language 49:519–536, 2003), but how ubiquitous is this phenomenon? In two experiments, we investigated whether this tendency to encode and rely on inaccuracies from text might be influenced by the plausibility of misinformation. In Experiment 1, we presented stories containing inaccurate plausible statements (e.g., “The Pilgrims’ ship was the Godspeed”), inaccurate implausible statements (e.g., . . . the Titanic), or accurate statements (e.g., . . . the Mayflower). On a subsequent test of general knowledge, participants relied significantly less on implausible than on plausible inaccuracies from the texts but continued to rely on accurate information. In Experiment 2, we replicated these results with the addition of a think-aloud procedure to elicit information about readers’ noticing and evaluative processes for plausible and implausible misinformation. Participants indicated more skepticism and less acceptance of implausible than of plausible inaccuracies. In contrast, they often failed to notice, completely ignored, and at times even explicitly accepted the misinformation provided by plausible lures. These results offer insight into the conditions under which reliance on inaccurate information occurs and suggest potential mechanisms that may underlie reported misinformation effects. 相似文献
305.
Jennifer M. Eastabrook Jessica J. Flynn Tom Hollenstein 《Journal of child and family studies》2014,23(3):487-496
The transition into adolescence involves a number of changes that for many adolescents result in increased negative affect and internalizing symptoms, especially for females. In the current study we examined the direct and indirect effects of emotional awareness on internalizing symptoms by exploring the extent to which certain emotion regulation strategies influence this relationship. Participants were 123 female adolescents aged 13–16 years (M = 14.51 years) who completed measures of emotional awareness, emotion regulation (emotional reappraisal and expressive suppression), and symptoms of depression and social anxiety. Two multiple indirect effect models were conducted including both reappraisal and suppression (one for each of the dependent variables, depression and social anxiety) via the bootstrapping method. Results found that reappraisal accounted for the effect of emotional awareness on depressive symptoms but suppression accounted for the effect of emotional awareness on social anxiety symptoms. Results suggest that emotion regulation strategies play an important role in determining depressive and social anxiety symptoms and are associated with an adolescent’s level of emotional awareness. 相似文献
306.
Brittany Harding Rylan Egan Peter Kannu Jennifer J. MacKenzie 《Journal of genetic counseling》2017,26(3):541-547
Parents have the opportunity to educate their children to facilitate behaviours and lifestyle habits that may prevent or delay genetic disease, or mitigate predispositions within the family. We sought to determine parents’ understanding of genetic knowledge and heritability. Using a quantitative survey methodology 108 volunteer participants were surveyed from a convenience sample of all parents/caregivers within the waiting room of a general children’s outpatient clinic. Results indicated that average genetic knowledge levels were fairly high, with the majority of participants scoring 70–80 % correct on knowledge-based questions. Further, scores were found to be positively correlated with education, but inversely correlated with self-perceived knowledge. This finding suggests that participants with less experience tended to overestimate their knowledge. We suggest that gaps in knowledge of genetics and heritability could be improved by using educational interventions such as media campaigns, provision of informational brochures, or changes to current high school curriculum which would increase exposure to genetics and heritability for both parents and children. 相似文献
307.
308.
309.
Jennifer E. Swanberg Sharon P. McKechnie Mamta U. Ojha Jacquelyn B. James 《Journal of Vocational Behavior》2011,79(3):613-624
The changing natures of both work and the lives of the U.S. workforce have created an array of challenges for organizations attempting to foster work engagement. To accommodate the work and family needs of an increasingly diverse workforce, many firms are offering flexible work solutions to employees. However, the distribution of these types of organizational resources is unequal, with workers in lower-wage hourly jobs having the least access to any form of flexibility. The purpose of this paper is to examine the relationship of schedule control, as a form of flexible work practice, and work engagement among workers employed in hourly retail jobs. Authors tested a model whereby the relationship between schedule control and work engagement is mediated by schedule satisfaction and perceived supervisor support. A sample of 1343 full-time hourly retail workers was used to address the study's purpose. Hypothesized relationships were fully or partially supported. Implications for research and practice are discussed. 相似文献
310.
Ryan Williams LaMothe 《Pastoral Psychology》2011,60(4):523-536
In this article, I offer a way of thinking about the interplay of political and religious subjectivities in persons’ conversion
experiences or faith journeys. More particularly, I contend that a person’s political and religious experiences interact such
that political experiences and ideas shape the person’s religious values and beliefs, and vice versa. Malcolm X’s story is
used to illustrate this interplay of subjectivities vis-à-vis the varied changes or conversions in his life. The major point
here is that the richness and depth of change in Malcolm X’s life (and other figures) are better grasped when we take into
account the dynamic intersection of the political and religious. 相似文献