全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4092篇 |
免费 | 55篇 |
专业分类
4147篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 27篇 |
2023年 | 35篇 |
2022年 | 33篇 |
2021年 | 66篇 |
2020年 | 80篇 |
2019年 | 114篇 |
2018年 | 172篇 |
2017年 | 180篇 |
2016年 | 175篇 |
2015年 | 118篇 |
2014年 | 139篇 |
2013年 | 447篇 |
2012年 | 257篇 |
2011年 | 259篇 |
2010年 | 132篇 |
2009年 | 161篇 |
2008年 | 239篇 |
2007年 | 230篇 |
2006年 | 177篇 |
2005年 | 154篇 |
2004年 | 156篇 |
2003年 | 155篇 |
2002年 | 134篇 |
2001年 | 49篇 |
2000年 | 43篇 |
1999年 | 44篇 |
1998年 | 52篇 |
1997年 | 42篇 |
1996年 | 39篇 |
1995年 | 35篇 |
1994年 | 23篇 |
1993年 | 28篇 |
1992年 | 17篇 |
1991年 | 14篇 |
1990年 | 9篇 |
1989年 | 14篇 |
1988年 | 7篇 |
1987年 | 9篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 8篇 |
1984年 | 9篇 |
1983年 | 9篇 |
1982年 | 5篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 5篇 |
1979年 | 7篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
1974年 | 5篇 |
1973年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有4147条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
831.
832.
Jennifer Iannuzzi 《Journal of genetic counseling》2012,21(6):791-792
833.
Jennifer J. Freyd 《Cognitive Science》1983,7(3):191-210
Psychologists and cognitive scientists interested in the nature of internal representations of human knowledge often use observable regularities or structures to infer what the innate constraints on those representations must be like. It is possible, however, that certain structures might come about only when a group of people share a knowledge domain. Furthermore, it is possible that there are analyzable constraints on knowledge structures that emerge when knowledge is being shared. Such constraints are referred to in this paper as “shareability” constraints. A number of examples of observable structures in human knowledge are discussed in terms of shareability constraints. An attempt is made to determine which sorts of structures are most shareable, and how those structures may differ from the sorts of structures that are easily represented by the individual mind but not easily shared between minds. 相似文献
834.
835.
836.
837.
Correct running form is important for injury prevention; as such, correct form promotes continued engagement in running as a long-term form of exercise. Researchers have shown video feedback to be an effective strategy to improve athletic form for a variety of sports, but it has not been evaluated in any behavior analytic research as a method for improving running form. The purpose of this study is to evaluate the effectiveness of video feedback to improve running form for regular runners. During baseline, each participant was video recorded while running without any feedback (verbal or video). During intervention, the researcher recorded the participant, then showed them the video and provided feedback on correct or incorrect form, according to a 9-step task analysis. Results show that all three participants achieved 100% correct steps on the task analysis when video feedback was used to maintain their form during follow-up. 相似文献
838.
839.
Jessica L. Paranczak Joseph M. Lambert Jennifer R. Ledford Bailey A. Copeland M. Janey Macdonald 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》2024,57(2):408-425
Recommendations for achieving generalized instructional outcomes often overlook the capacity for generative learning for most verbally competent humans. Four children (ages 5–8) participated in this project. In Study 1, we provided decontextualized discrete trial teaching to establish arbitrary relations between colors, pictures of characters, and researcher motor actions. All participants engaged in derivative responding, providing evidence of relational framing. Subsequently, we demonstrated that, with no additional instruction, these derivatives contributed to effective action within a socially valid context (i.e., Candyland gameplay). Study 2 extended the demonstration by teaching frames of opposition. Following teaching, all participants engaged in novel and contextually appropriate responding that entailed the derivation of both coordination and opposition between untrained stimuli. This outcome demonstrates how teaching simple relations can result in learning that manifests at higher levels of complexity (i.e., relational networking), providing some evidence that there can be socially valid benefits to decontextualized discrete trial instruction. 相似文献
840.
Claudia Campos Sarah E. Bloom Lori Kollin Jennifer R. Weyman 《Behavioral Interventions》2023,38(2):302-316
The extent to which multiple schedules are an effective schedule thinning method following functional communication training (FCT) relies on the control the schedule-correlated stimuli exert over behavior. Thus, the stimuli used to signal the schedule in place (e.g., reinforcement and extinction) in a multiple schedule arrangement require special attention. To date, the majority of the research on multiple schedules has evaluated the use of different arbitrary signals as schedule-correlated stimuli (e.g., poster boards). These signals are considered static as they lack movement. More recently, some studies have successfully used dynamic signals, which include movement or animation, within multiple schedule arrangements. However, the extent to which one type of signal may result in faster stimulus control over behavior has not been evaluated. Thus, the purpose of this study was to compare the use of static and dynamic signals as schedule-correlated stimuli in multiple schedules used within the context of FCT. Four children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder participated in the study. The results suggest that no differences in discriminated manding were observed for three out of four participants. Only dynamic signals resulted in discriminated manding for one participant. 相似文献