全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3762篇 |
免费 | 215篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
3979篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 7篇 |
2023年 | 32篇 |
2022年 | 31篇 |
2021年 | 62篇 |
2020年 | 77篇 |
2019年 | 111篇 |
2018年 | 171篇 |
2017年 | 180篇 |
2016年 | 167篇 |
2015年 | 116篇 |
2014年 | 132篇 |
2013年 | 432篇 |
2012年 | 253篇 |
2011年 | 254篇 |
2010年 | 129篇 |
2009年 | 155篇 |
2008年 | 233篇 |
2007年 | 227篇 |
2006年 | 169篇 |
2005年 | 152篇 |
2004年 | 154篇 |
2003年 | 155篇 |
2002年 | 128篇 |
2001年 | 46篇 |
2000年 | 42篇 |
1999年 | 40篇 |
1998年 | 49篇 |
1997年 | 39篇 |
1996年 | 34篇 |
1995年 | 33篇 |
1994年 | 23篇 |
1993年 | 26篇 |
1992年 | 11篇 |
1991年 | 8篇 |
1990年 | 7篇 |
1989年 | 10篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 9篇 |
1986年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 8篇 |
1984年 | 7篇 |
1983年 | 7篇 |
1982年 | 4篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 5篇 |
1976年 | 4篇 |
1975年 | 4篇 |
1974年 | 3篇 |
1968年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有3979条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
991.
The cultural intelligence hypothesis (CIH) claims that humans’ advanced cognition is a direct result of human culture and that children are uniquely specialized to absorb and utilize this cultural experience ( Tomasello, 2000 ). Comparative data demonstrating that 2.5‐year‐old human children outperform apes on measures of social cognition but not on measures of physical cognition support this claim ( Herrmann et al., 2007 ). However, the previous study failed to control for rearing when comparing these two species. Specifically, the human children were raised in a human culture whereas the apes were raised in standard sanctuary settings. To further explore the CIH, here we compared the performance on multiple measures of social and physical cognition in a group of standard reared apes raised in conditions typical of zoo and biomedical laboratory settings to that of apes reared in an enculturated socio‐communicatively rich environment. Overall, the enculturated apes significantly outperformed their standard reared counterparts on the cognitive tasks and this was particularly true for measures of communication. Furthermore, the performance of the enculturated apes was very similar to previously reported data from 2.5‐year‐old children. We conclude that apes who are reared in a human‐like socio‐communicatively rich environment develop superior communicative abilities compared to apes reared in standard laboratory settings, which supports some assumptions of the cultural intelligence hypothesis. 相似文献
992.
This critique proposes that self-forgiveness is a misleading and inaccurate concept for understanding the conditions to which
it is applied. Besides the fact that traditional religion provides no rationale for self-forgiveness, four specific criticisms
are presented. (1) Self-forgiveness causes splitting of the self—creating various problems. (2) It involves a conflict of
interest between the self that judges and the self that is judged. (3) Through its extreme emphasis on the self, it promotes
narcissism and appeals to narcissists. (4) Research indicates that interpersonal forgiveness and self or intrapersonal forgiveness
involve different psychological processes. We conclude that self-acceptance is a more accurate and useful term for the process
and benefits attributed to self-forgiveness. 相似文献
993.
Perfectionistic Automatic Thoughts,Trait Perfectionism,and Bulimic Automatic Thoughts in Young Women
Gordon L. Flett Jennifer Newby Paul L. Hewitt Christina Persaud 《Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy》2011,29(3):192-206
The current research examined the associations among perfectionistic automatic thoughts, trait perfectionism, negative automatic
thoughts, and bulimic automatic thoughts. A non-clinical sample of 94 undergraduate women completed the Perfectionism Cognitions
Inventory, the Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale, the Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire, and the Bulimic Automatic Thoughts
Test. Correlational tests revealed that two automatic thoughts measures (perfectionistic automatic thoughts and negative automatic
thoughts) and two trait perfectionism measures (self-oriented and socially prescribed perfectionism) were associated significantly
with bulimic automatic thoughts with the strongest association being between perfectionistic automatic thoughts and bulimic
automatic thoughts. Regression analyses showed that perfectionistic thoughts predicted unique variance in bulimic thoughts
beyond variance attributable to trait perfectionism and negative automatic thoughts. The findings are discussed in terms of
the need to incorporate an explicit focus on perfectionistic automatic thoughts into treatment interventions and conceptual
models of perfectionism and eating disorders. 相似文献
994.
Otis JD McGlinchey R Vasterling JJ Kerns RD 《Journal of clinical psychology in medical settings》2011,18(2):145-154
The nature of combat in Iraq and Afghanistan has resulted in high rates of comorbidity among chronic pain, posttraumatic stress
disorder (PTSD), and mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI) in Veterans of Operations Enduring Freedom and Iraqi Freedom (OEF/OIF).
Although separate evidence-based psychological treatments have been developed for chronic pain and PTSD, far less is known
about how to approach treatment when these conditions co-occur, and especially when they co-occur with mTBI. To provide the
best care possible for OEF/OIF Veterans, clinicians need to have a clearer understanding of how to identify these conditions,
ways in which these conditions may interact with one another, and ways in which existing evidence-based treatments can be
modified to meet the needs of individuals with mTBI. The purpose of the present paper is to review the comorbidity of pain,
PTSD, and mTBI in OEF/OIF Veterans, and provide recommendations to clinicians who provide care to Veterans with these conditions.
First, we will begin with an overview of the presentation, symptomatology, and treatment of chronic pain and PTSD. The challenges
associated with mTBI in OEF/OIF Veterans will be reported and data will be presented on the comorbidity among all three of
these conditions in OEF/OIF Veterans. Second, we will present recommendations for providing psychological treatment for chronic
pain and PTSD when comorbid with mTBI. Finally, the paper concludes with a discussion of the need for a multidisciplinary
treatment approach, as well as a call for continued research to further refine existing treatments for these conditions. 相似文献
995.
Vaidya CJ Foss-Feig J Shook D Kaplan L Kenworthy L Gaillard WD 《Developmental science》2011,14(4):911-924
Functional magnetic resonance imaging was used to examine functional anatomy of attention to social (eye gaze) and nonsocial (arrow) communicative stimuli in late childhood and in a disorder defined by atypical processing of social stimuli, Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD). Children responded to a target word ('LEFT'/'RIGHT') in the context of a distracting arrow or averted gaze pointing in a direction that was congruent, incongruent, or neutral (bar without arrowheads, central gaze) relative to the target word. Despite being irrelevant to the target task, both arrow and averted gaze facilitated responses (Congruent vs. Neutral trials) to the same extent in the two groups and led to interference (Incongruent vs. Congruent trials), which was greater from arrows in ASD than control children. In the brain, interaction between group and distracter-domain was observed in frontal-temporal regions during facilitation and frontal-striatal regions during interference. During facilitation, regions associated with attention to gaze in control children (left superior temporal sulcus, premotor) were associated with attention to arrows in ASD children; gaze was associated with medial temporal involvement in ASD children. During interference, regions associated with arrows in control children (anterior cingulate, right caudate) were activated in response to gaze in ASD children; further, left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, a region not observed in control children, was activated during gaze-interference in ASD children. Thus, functional anatomy was atypical in ASD children during spontaneous processing of social and nonsocial communicative cues. 相似文献
996.
Borton JL Reiner DR Vazquez EV Ruddiman JF Anglin SM 《The Journal of social psychology》2011,151(3):240-256
In the current study, we examined the effects of women's suppressing negative gender stereotypes while interacting with a male confederate. Compared with control participants, those who suppressed negative thoughts about women's ability experienced less self-confidence, lower self-esteem, and were more nonverbally submissive during the interaction, particularly if they were high in stigma consciousness (Pinel, 1999). These findings illustrate the negative intra- and interpersonal consequences of stigma suppression. 相似文献
997.
Kraus MW Horberg EJ Goetz JL Keltner D 《Personality & social psychology bulletin》2011,37(10):1376-1388
Lower-class individuals, because of their lower rank in society, are theorized to be more vigilant to social threats relative to their high-ranking upper-class counterparts. This class-related vigilance to threat, the authors predicted, would shape the emotional content of social interactions in systematic ways. In Study 1, participants engaged in a teasing interaction with a close friend. Lower-class participants--measured in terms of social class rank in society and within the friendship--more accurately tracked the hostile emotions of their friend. As a result, lower-class individuals experienced more hostile emotion contagion relative to upper-class participants. In Study 2, lower-class participants manipulated to experience lower subjective socioeconomic rank showed more hostile reactivity to ambiguous social scenarios relative to upper-class participants and to lower-class participants experiencing elevated socioeconomic rank. The results suggest that class affects expectations, perception, and experience of hostile emotion, particularly in situations in which lower-class individuals perceive their subordinate rank. 相似文献
998.
Fixed momentary schedules of differential reinforcement of other behavior (FM DRO) generally have been ineffective as treatment for problem behavior. Because most early research on FM DRO included presentation of a signal at the end of the DRO interval, it is unclear whether the limited effects of FM DRO were due to (a) the momentary response requirement of the schedule per se or (b) discrimination of the contingency made more salient by the signal. To separate these two potential influences, we compared the effects of signaled versus unsignaled FM DRO with 4 individuals with developmental disabilities whose problem behavior was maintained by social-positive reinforcement. During signaled FM DRO, the experimenter presented a visual stimulus 3 s prior to the end of the DRO interval and delivered reinforcement contingent on the absence of problem behavior at the second the interval elapsed. Unsignaled DRO was identical except that interval termination was not signaled. Results indicated that signaled FM DRO was effective in decreasing 2 subjects' problem behavior, whereas an unsignaled schedule was required for the remaining 2 subjects. These results suggest that the response requirement per se of FM DRO may not be problematic if it is not easily discriminated. 相似文献
999.
Ganiban JM Ulbricht J Saudino KJ Reiss D Neiderhiser JM 《Developmental psychology》2011,47(3):676-692
The degree to which child temperament moderates genetic and environmental contributions to parenting was examined. Participants were drawn from the Nonshared Environment and Adolescent Development project and included 720 sibling pairs, ages 13.5 + 2.0 years (Sibling 1) to 12.1 + 1.3 years (Sibling 2). The sample consisted of 6 sibling types: 93 monozygotic twin pairs, 99 dizygotic twin pairs, and 95 full sibling pairs from never-divorced families and 182 full-sibling, 109 half-sibling, and 130 unrelated-sibling pairs residing in stepfamilies. Composite child temperament ratings (negative emotionality, activity, shyness, and sociability) were derived from mothers' and fathers' reports. Composite parenting ratings (negativity, warmth) for mothers and fathers were generated from children's and parents' reports. Analyses indicated that at higher levels of negative emotionality and sociability, child-based genetic contributions to mothers' and fathers' negativity increased, whereas the contributions of environmental factors declined. The opposite pattern was observed for child shyness. These same characteristics had less impact on parental warmth. For fathers only, nonshared environmental contributions to fathers' warmth increased in the presence of high child activity and sociability but declined when children were very shy. Overall these findings indicate that child-based effects on negative parenting are enhanced when children demonstrate potentially challenging characteristics but are weaker in the absence of such characteristics. 相似文献
1000.
Comparison is one of the most ubiquitous and versatile components in human information processing. Social comparisons in particular have multifaceted judgmental, affective, behavioral, and motivational consequences. Initially, the consequences of nonsocial comparisons may appear to be less complex than the consequences of social comparisons. Comparing a smaller circle with a larger circle, for example, has little motivational relevance. Across two studies, however, we demonstrated that different types of nonsocial comparisons influence the motivational underpinnings of participants' subsequent performance. Participants who were perceptually or numerically induced to compare upward (e.g., to compare a smaller stimulus with a larger stimulus) in a first task traded accuracy for speed in a subsequent unrelated performance task. Conversely, participants who had compared downward traded speed for accuracy. These trade-offs are indicative of promotion- and prevention-focused motivational orientations, respectively. Comparing a geometric circle with a "superior" (e.g., larger) circle seems to yield promotion-focused strategic orientations similar to those produced by comparing oneself to a person one perceives as superior. These findings demonstrate the surprising motivational power of nonsocial comparisons. 相似文献