全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3917篇 |
免费 | 244篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
4163篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 6篇 |
2023年 | 32篇 |
2022年 | 31篇 |
2021年 | 64篇 |
2020年 | 76篇 |
2019年 | 115篇 |
2018年 | 175篇 |
2017年 | 186篇 |
2016年 | 173篇 |
2015年 | 116篇 |
2014年 | 131篇 |
2013年 | 443篇 |
2012年 | 261篇 |
2011年 | 261篇 |
2010年 | 133篇 |
2009年 | 157篇 |
2008年 | 235篇 |
2007年 | 231篇 |
2006年 | 180篇 |
2005年 | 155篇 |
2004年 | 163篇 |
2003年 | 164篇 |
2002年 | 134篇 |
2001年 | 50篇 |
2000年 | 51篇 |
1999年 | 45篇 |
1998年 | 53篇 |
1997年 | 44篇 |
1996年 | 39篇 |
1995年 | 36篇 |
1994年 | 25篇 |
1993年 | 34篇 |
1992年 | 14篇 |
1991年 | 11篇 |
1990年 | 11篇 |
1989年 | 12篇 |
1988年 | 6篇 |
1987年 | 9篇 |
1986年 | 10篇 |
1985年 | 11篇 |
1984年 | 12篇 |
1983年 | 9篇 |
1982年 | 8篇 |
1981年 | 9篇 |
1980年 | 5篇 |
1979年 | 6篇 |
1978年 | 4篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
1974年 | 5篇 |
1969年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有4163条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
101.
Meenakshi Shukla Rakesh Pandey Jennifer Y. F. Lau 《International journal of psychology》2019,54(2):214-222
This study examined the effect of sense modality (auditory/visual) on emotional dampening (reduced responsiveness to emotions with elevation in blood pressure). Fifty‐six normotensive participants were assessed on tasks requiring labelling and matching of emotions in faces and voices. Based on median split of systolic and diastolic blood pressures (SBP and DBP, respectively), participants were divided into low BP, high BP and isolated BP groups. On emotion‐labelling tasks, analysis revealed reduced emotion recognition in the high BP than the low BP group. On emotion‐matching tasks, reduced emotion recognition was noted in high and also isolated BP group as compared to low BP group for the task that required matching a visual target with one of the four auditory distractors. Our findings show for the first time that even isolated elevations in either SBP or DBP may result in emotional dampening. Furthermore, the study highlights that the emotional dampening effect generalises to explicit processing (labelling) of emotional information in both faces and voices—and that these effects tentatively occur during more pragmatic and covert (matching) emotion recognition processes too. These findings require replication in clinical hypertensives. 相似文献
102.
Jennifer A. Herdt 《The Journal of religious ethics》2019,47(1):68-93
Eudaimonism is often regarded as egoistic. If it recommends that agents pursue their own good because it is their own good, it is guilty as charged. But excellence‐prior eudaimonism offers a non‐egoistic alternative to this welfare‐prior eudaimonism. Excellence‐prior eudaimonism recommends that an agent live in a way that is in fact good for the agent, but it does not regard the agent’s own good as necessarily that for the sake of which the agent acts, nor does it regard living well as justified by the fact that it is good for the agent, but simply because it is good. The Christian eudaimonisms of Augustine and Aquinas are best understood as deepened forms of excellence‐prior eudaimonism. 相似文献
103.
Although the percentage of Black-White interracial marriage continues to increase in the United States, research is limited related to the impact of stigma on these couples and the processes that influence their responses to negative racial experiences. In the current study, the Vulnerability-Stress-Adaptation model was used as a theoretical foundation to investigate the relationship between stigma and couple satisfaction among Black-White interracially married couples, along with the potential mediating role of religious/spiritual well-being. A community sample of 180 interracially married individuals responded to surveys assessing their experiences of stigma due to being a member of an interracial couple, religious/spiritual well-being, and couple satisfaction. The analysis revealed that relationship stigma was negatively associated with couple satisfaction, and religious/spiritual well-being mediated the relationship between these two variables. To conclude, recommendations for intervention and prevention in the lives of Black-White interracial couples are offered. 相似文献
104.
In experiment 1, pigeons were trained to match temporal (2, 8, and 10 s of houselight) and location (feeder light, left key,
center key illumination) samples to color comparison stimuli. Red choices were correct following the 2-s and feeder light,
orange choices were correct following the 8-s and center key, and green choices were correct following the 10-s and left key.
Samples that were harder to discriminate (8- vs 10-s, and left vs center key) were mapped onto comparisons that were easy
to discriminate (orange vs green), while samples that were easier to discriminate (2- vs 8-s, and feeder light vs left key)
were mapped onto comparisons that were hard to discriminate(red vs orange). The pattern of errors for temporal and location
samples indicated that these samples were not represented by a common code even though they were associated with the same
comparison stimuli. In experiment 2, the same pigeons were trained with visual samples in which samples that were hard to
discriminate (triangle vs circle) were mapped onto comparisons that were easy to discriminate (orange vs green), while samples
that were easy to discriminate(plus vs triangle) were mapped onto comparisons that were hard to discriminate (red vs orange).
Following acquisition of the visual discrimination, the temporal samples were re-introduced and many-to-one training was continued.
During delay testing, the pattern of errors for temporal and visual samples was equivalent and consistent with the hypothesis
that visual samples were being coded in terms of the duration appropriate for the temporal sample with which it shared a common
comparison response. Data from no-sample test sessions ruled out a simple response bias explanation of the data. The properties
of common codes for temporal and nontemporal events can be somewhat flexible and more complicated than previously envisaged.
Received: 21 January 1998 / Accepted after revision: 5 April 1998 相似文献
105.
The American Journal of Psychoanalysis - 相似文献
106.
Marilyn Lewis Lanza Jennifer Anderson Helene Satz James Stone Herbert L. Kayne Kathleen Smith Kathleen Rideout 《Group》1998,22(1):15-37
An observational scale to record aggressive behaviors, defenses, and interventions that occur during a psychotherapy group has been developed. Content and process items were included. The content items focus on the spoken material and are on a continuum of increasing group members' awareness of and responsibility for their anger. The process items are directed to underlying psychodynamic issues on a continuum from avoiding anger, to indirect and then expression of anger. Leader interventions (responses) are also included. The development of the scale, the Aggression Observation Scale for Group Psychotherapy (AOSGP), is described including pilot testing and reliability and validity findings. 相似文献
107.
Jennifer Dorfman 《Memory & cognition》1998,26(6):1157-1172
This study provides further evidence for the notion, proposed by Dorfman (1994), that activation of sublexical components plays an important role in implicit memory for novel words. Priming in an identification task was observed consistently when nonwords were formed out of morphemes; weaker, more variable effects were observed when nonwords were composed of syllables or pseudosyllables. Priming was insensitive to changes in the surface features (type case) of the stimuli and was disrupted by rearrangement of the stimulus components. Results are interpreted in terms of the activation and integration of preexisting sublexical representations and as evidence against the role of newly established episodic or perceptual representations. It is concluded that findings of priming for novel information should not be taken as evidence against an activation view. 相似文献
108.
Eric S. Davis Jennifer K. Pereira Andrea Dixon 《Journal of Creativity in Mental Health》2015,10(4):402-422
Many elementary school students lack school relationships required for positive personal, social, and academic interactions. Reality therapy and play therapy both seek to address these relationships through developmentally appropriate and effective interventions. Integrating these 2 approaches has the potential for elementary school counselors to provide students with opportunities to create positive relationships and develop problem-solving skills. In this study, 8 school counselors participated in a series of trainings introducing reality play-therapy techniques to assess this potential. A thematic analysis conducted on feedback sessions following the trainings yielded themes related to the participants’ positive perceptions to the training and possible application. 相似文献
109.
Forgive them for I have sinned: The relationship between guilt and forgiveness of others' transgressions 下载免费PDF全文
Jennifer Jordan Francis J. Flynn Taya R. Cohen 《European journal of social psychology》2015,45(4):441-459
We propose that guilt leads to forgiveness of others' transgressions. In Study 1, people prone to experience guilt (but not shame) were also prone to forgive others for past misdeeds. In Study 2, we manipulated harm‐ and inequity‐based guilt; both increased forgiveness of others' transgressions. Further, the effect of guilt on forgiveness was mediated by identification with the transgressor. In Study 3, we replicated the guilt–forgiveness relationship and examined three other plausible mediators: capability for similar wrongdoing, empathic understanding, and general identification; only identification with the transgressor satisfied the criteria for mediation. In Study 4, we induced guilt by asking participants to harm a friend or stranger. Guilt induced by harming a friend led to greater forgiveness of third‐party transgressors, and again, identification with the transgressor mediated the effect. We discuss the implications of these results for understanding how the prosocial effects of guilt extend beyond the boundaries of a single interpersonal relationship. 相似文献
110.
Longitudinal trajectories of bicultural identity integration in recently immigrated Hispanic adolescents: Links with mental health and family functioning 下载免费PDF全文
Seth J. Schwartz Jennifer B. Unger Lourdes Baezconde‐Garbanati Verónica Benet‐Martínez Alan Meca Byron L. Zamboanga Elma I. Lorenzo‐Blanco Sabrina E. Des Rosiers Assaf Oshri Raha F. Sabet Daniel W. Soto Monica Pattarroyo Shi Huang Juan A. Villamar Karina M. Lizzi José Szapocznik 《International journal of psychology》2015,50(6):440-450
This study examined, in a sample of recently immigrated Hispanic adolescents in Miami and Los Angeles, the extent to which bicultural identity integration (BII; involving the ability to synthesise one's heritage and receiving cultural streams and to identify as a member of both cultures) is best understood as a developmental construct that changes over time or as an individual‐difference construct that is largely stable over time. We were also interested in the extent to which these trajectories predicted mental health and family functioning. Recent‐immigrant 9th graders (N = 302) were assessed 6 times from 9th to 12th grade. Latent class growth analyses using the first 5 timepoints identified 2 trajectory classes—one with lower BII scores over time and another with higher BII scores over time. Higher heritage and US identity at baseline predicted membership in the higher BII class. At the 6th study timepoint, lower BII adolescents reported significantly poorer self‐esteem, optimism, prosocial behaviour and family relationships compared with their higher BII counterparts. These findings are discussed in terms of further research on the over‐time trajectory of biculturalism, and on the need to develop interventions to promote BII as a way of facilitating well‐being and positive family functioning. 相似文献