全文获取类型
收费全文 | 6257篇 |
免费 | 356篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
6615篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 54篇 |
2022年 | 54篇 |
2021年 | 103篇 |
2020年 | 138篇 |
2019年 | 178篇 |
2018年 | 252篇 |
2017年 | 280篇 |
2016年 | 263篇 |
2015年 | 187篇 |
2014年 | 216篇 |
2013年 | 755篇 |
2012年 | 394篇 |
2011年 | 365篇 |
2010年 | 221篇 |
2009年 | 232篇 |
2008年 | 315篇 |
2007年 | 313篇 |
2006年 | 244篇 |
2005年 | 237篇 |
2004年 | 223篇 |
2003年 | 210篇 |
2002年 | 189篇 |
2001年 | 81篇 |
2000年 | 84篇 |
1999年 | 98篇 |
1998年 | 82篇 |
1997年 | 66篇 |
1996年 | 60篇 |
1995年 | 53篇 |
1994年 | 43篇 |
1993年 | 52篇 |
1992年 | 40篇 |
1991年 | 31篇 |
1990年 | 25篇 |
1989年 | 33篇 |
1988年 | 21篇 |
1987年 | 34篇 |
1986年 | 25篇 |
1985年 | 31篇 |
1984年 | 19篇 |
1983年 | 19篇 |
1982年 | 23篇 |
1981年 | 16篇 |
1980年 | 18篇 |
1979年 | 27篇 |
1978年 | 15篇 |
1976年 | 17篇 |
1974年 | 21篇 |
1973年 | 20篇 |
1971年 | 17篇 |
排序方式: 共有6615条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
72.
Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: the mobilization-minimization hypothesis 总被引:23,自引:0,他引:23
S E Taylor 《Psychological bulletin》1991,110(1):67-85
Negative (adverse or threatening) events evoke strong and rapid physiological, cognitive, emotional, and social responses. This mobilization of the organism is followed by physiological, cognitive, and behavioral responses that damp down, minimize, and even erase the impact of that event. This pattern of mobilization-minimization appears to be greater for negative events than for neutral or positive events. Theoretical accounts of this response pattern are reviewed. It is concluded that no single theoretical mechanism can explain the mobilization-minimization pattern, but that a family of integrated process models, encompassing different classes of responses, may account for this pattern of parallel but disparately caused effects. 相似文献
73.
The present investigation focused upon the impact of gender and various powerful and less powerful linguistic features on the communication of unfamiliar male and female college students. Same-sex and mixed-sex dyads were recorded as they engaged in discussions regarding suggestions for a bogus freshman orientation booklet. Although few gender differences were found, the result of a factor analysis yielded three factors: questions, suggestions, and process variables. These factors are consistent with a power perspective in the use of various linguistic features. However, the present findings suggest that gender and power alone cannot explain differences in communication styles. Rather, the results suggest the need to consider other aspects of the speaker to explain differences that occur in the use of language. 相似文献
74.
William P. Hetrick Robert C. Isenhart Derek V. Taylor Curt A. Sandman 《Behavior research methods》1991,23(1):66-71
A software program written to collect real-time, observational data is described. The flexible program allows customized behavior codes and observational durations and simultaneously records both timed and counted events. The data are collected by means of single keystrokes, automatically stored to disk with 100th of a second resolution and summarized for each observational session. The program’s database files are dBASE III PLUS compatible and may be browsed, edited, or converted to ASCII files from the program’s main menu. Field testing demonstrated the efficiency and interobserver reliability of the program (for frequency,r=0.81; for durational behaviors,rs=0.89 and 0.96). The software operates on IBM XT/AT/PS 2s and most clones with PC/MS DOS version 2.0 or greater. 相似文献
75.
Bruce E. Compas Howard S. Adelman Pamela C. Freundl Perry Nelson Linda Taylor 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1982,10(1):77-83
Attributions made by children and their parents for the cause of the child's clinical problem were monitored during assessment interviews. Results support previously observed differences obtained through questionnaires, with parents making more attributions than their children to characteristics of the child. This pattern was affected by variations in interview format. Parents and children differed in the locus of their attributions when interviewed individually, but these differences were not present when families were interviewed with both parents and children present. Implications for the methodology of attribution research with child-clinical populations are highlighted. 相似文献
76.
Susan T. Fiske David A. Kenny Shelley E. Taylor 《Journal of experimental social psychology》1982,18(2):105-127
Previous research has demonstrated that salient information is overrepresented in causal attributions. Two experiments were conducted to investigate potential mediators for this effect and to make a case for the use of structural models in explanations of process. Two mediators were considered: enhanced visual recall of salient stimuli and exaggerated schema-relevant recall of salient stimuli. Although analyses of variance supported the visual recall model, structural analyses demonstrated its implausibility. Analyses of variance and structural models revealed that schema-relevant recall, that is, information seen as representative of causal influence, is a plausible mediator; this was particularly true of relevant visual information. Results suggest that salience effects (S. E. Taylor & S. T. Fiske in L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 11). New York: Academic Press, 1978) are due to: (a) the attentional advantage of inherently salient visual events and (b) the influence of stored visual and nonvisual schema-relevant information on causal judgments. 相似文献
77.
It is postulated that adult vocational maturity needs to be assessed because of the changing developmental tasks encountered during the course of a career. Vocational maturity having been found to be multidimensional, existing undimensional inventories of adult vocational maturity are considered of limited although demonstrated validity. Relevant theories of adult vocational development and adjustment are reviewed. Super's proposed adult vocational maturity model is examined for the promise that it offers and for the problems that arise in developing a multidimensional measure of vocational maturity. A way of surmounting these problems is proposed as a basis for work now being undertaken. 相似文献
78.
Matthew M. Burg Dennis H. Reid Jennifer Lattimore 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1979,12(3):363-375
The use of a self-recording and supervision program to increase interactions between direct care staff and profoundly retarded persons in a state residential facility was investigated. Following baseline, staff were provided with instructions regarding what to self-record, criteria for how many interactions to record, and a prepared card on which to make the recordings. Throughout the study, the staff supervisor monitored intermittently staff-client interactions. Observations indicated that when the staff recorded their interactions with clients in a loosely structured dayroom setting, the rate of interactions increased noticeably for each staff person. Behavioral ecology measures indicated that other staff responsibilities, such as maintaining the cleanliness of residents and the physical area, were not affected detrimentally when social interactions increased and actually showed small improvements. Additionally, small decreases in resident self-stimulatory and disruptive/aggressive behaviors occurred when the rate of social interactions from staff persons increased. Follow-up measures indicated that the rate of staff self-recording was variable, but when staff did self-record, the increased rate of staff-client interactions maintained. 相似文献
79.
80.
Susan T. Fiske Shelley E. Taylor Nancy L. Etcoff Jessica K. Laufer 《Journal of experimental social psychology》1979,15(4):356-377
Three studies were conducted to test whether imagery accounts for the effects of empathy on attributions, that is, whether attributers construct and scan a mental image of social scenarios the same way that actual participants scan a real environment. According to this interpretation, attributers “see” the world as the participant does and make vicarious attributions. An alternative interpretation holds that subjects base vicarious attributions on recreating the motivations and affect of the actor, so that imaging is irrelevant to empathy and attributions. In the first study, subjects who imagined a story from the perspective of a particular character later showed differential recall of story details as a function of role, but not differential attributions. In Experiment 2, role-taking subjects showed clear effects of imaged perspective on recall for story details, but no effects on attributions of causality for an accident. Further, recall and attribution were uncorrelated. In the last study, empathy and imagery role-taking instructions produced independent effects: imagers showed pronounced perspective-relevant recall and empathizers did not. Neither showed unambiguous vicarious attributions. Recall and attribution were again uncorrelated. These studies suggest that the imagery explanation of empathy effects is untenable, and imply that the recall of perspective-relevant details is unlikely to mediate attributions of causality in imaginary scenarios. 相似文献