全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2779篇 |
免费 | 33篇 |
专业分类
2812篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 18篇 |
2021年 | 23篇 |
2020年 | 40篇 |
2019年 | 47篇 |
2018年 | 65篇 |
2017年 | 80篇 |
2016年 | 98篇 |
2015年 | 53篇 |
2014年 | 70篇 |
2013年 | 273篇 |
2012年 | 117篇 |
2011年 | 129篇 |
2010年 | 82篇 |
2009年 | 85篇 |
2008年 | 135篇 |
2007年 | 137篇 |
2006年 | 136篇 |
2005年 | 107篇 |
2004年 | 99篇 |
2003年 | 115篇 |
2002年 | 103篇 |
2001年 | 45篇 |
2000年 | 48篇 |
1999年 | 30篇 |
1998年 | 39篇 |
1997年 | 36篇 |
1996年 | 38篇 |
1995年 | 27篇 |
1994年 | 23篇 |
1993年 | 13篇 |
1992年 | 25篇 |
1991年 | 22篇 |
1990年 | 21篇 |
1989年 | 28篇 |
1988年 | 33篇 |
1987年 | 22篇 |
1986年 | 22篇 |
1985年 | 28篇 |
1984年 | 37篇 |
1983年 | 19篇 |
1982年 | 22篇 |
1981年 | 20篇 |
1980年 | 29篇 |
1979年 | 20篇 |
1978年 | 17篇 |
1977年 | 16篇 |
1976年 | 27篇 |
1975年 | 12篇 |
1974年 | 16篇 |
1973年 | 16篇 |
排序方式: 共有2812条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
151.
Jeffrey B. Adams Jerome Adams Robert W. Rice Debra Instone 《Journal of applied social psychology》1985,15(5):387-398
Cadets at the U.S. Military Academy completed a questionnaire describing their experiences in Cadet Basic Training (CBT) and Cadet Field Training (CFT). Analyses of variance examined the impact of Group Role (Leader vs. Follower) and Perceived Group Effectiveness (High vs. Low) on group member perceptions of the extent to which each of six factors affected group performance. Significant main effects for Perceived Effectiveness were interpreted in terms of the egodefensive bias. Role main effects indicated that leaders tended to make stronger dispositional attributions than did followers. There were no signifcant Group Role × Perceived Effectiveness interaction effects. Findings support, in part, hypotheses concerning the operation of an attributional bias within the leader-member relationship. 相似文献
152.
Haddad JM Rietdyk S Ryu JH Seaman JM Silver TA Kalish JA Hughes CM 《Journal of motor behavior》2011,43(4):345-355
The authors examined postural asymmetries during quiet stance and while holding evenly or unevenly distributed loads. Right-hand dominant subjects preferentially loaded their right lower limb when holding no load or a load evenly distributed in both hands, but no differences in center of pressure (CoP) were observed between the left and right limbs. However, longer CoP displacement was observed under the preferentially loaded limb, which may reflect a functional asymmetry that allows quick movement of one limb in response to a potential perturbation. When a load was held only in the nondominant hand, sample entropy decreased in the left (loaded) limb but increased in the right (unloaded) limb, suggesting the unloaded foot compensated for a loss of control flexibility in the loaded foot. 相似文献
153.
Thomas G. Adams Josh M. Cisler Robert E. Brady Jeffrey M. Lohr Bunmi O. Olatunji 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2013,35(3):375-388
Contamination aversion is implicated in a range of psychological disorders, such as obsessive-compulsive disorder and health anxiety. Despite broad implications, the core mechanisms mediating contamination aversion are mixed and have not clearly been elucidated. The present studies are psychometric investigations of the components of contamination aversion and tests of the different mechanisms that mediate the severity of these components. In Study 1, items from existing measures of contamination aversion were compiled into one measure and administered to a large sample of nonclinical participants (N?=?1,286). Exploratory factor analyses demonstrated a two-factor structure, with one factor comprised of items related to intrinsically (direct) contaminated objects and the other factor comprised of items related to extrinsically (indirect) contaminated objects. In Study 2, items were added and confirmatory factor analyses supported a two-factor model in another large nonclinical sample (N?=?612). In Study 3 (N?=?471), structural modeling demonstrated distinct relations between the two factors and affective and cognitive constructs. Affective constructs related more to aversions toward more directly contaminated objects and situations while cognitive constructs related more to aversions toward more indirectly contaminated objects and situations. The present studies help clarify and articulate the unique roles of cognition and affect in specific contamination aversions, and, as such, have important implications for conceptual modeling of contamination-related psychopathology. 相似文献
154.
Jeffrey J. Martin 《Journal of Loss and Trauma》2013,18(3):225-230
Abstract Sport scientists have started to amass a small body of knowledge on loss in sport. Viewing sport retirement from a loss framework is particularly appropriate when examining transitions out of disability sport because athletes with disabilities are at risk for facing unique losses when they lease sport (Martin, 1996; Wheeler, Malone, & Steadward, 1996). The purpose of this article is to discuss loss experiences in disability sport from psychological, social, and physiological perspectives. 相似文献
155.
Edward McAuley Bryan Blissmer Jeffrey Katula Terry E. Duncan 《Psychology & health》2013,28(3):341-355
Abstract Increasing evidence suggests that regular physical activity can have considerable psychological, as well as physical, benefits in the elderly. Although factors such as exercise dosage may be implicated in exercise-induced affect responses, it has also been suggested that social and psychological factors might influence this relationship. This study examined the roles played by exercise environment (group versus alone) and self-efficacy in affective change in 80 older adults (M age = 66 yrs) over the course of three acute exercise bouts. Using latent growth curve methodology and statistically controlling for duration and intensity of exercise, we were able to demonstrate that social (group) environments resulted in statistically significant improvements in feeling state responses when contrasted with a condition in which the participants exercised alone. In addition, increases in self-efficacy were associated with more positive and less negative feeling states. Environmental factors that might influence the exercise-affect response are discussed and recommendations for subsequent exercise, efficacy, affect research made. 相似文献
156.
Abstract Thirty-seven 3-year-old children, who had learned a 9-action event sequence (“making Play-Doh spaghetti”) when they were 20 months old, returned to the lab to determine whether they would be able to verbally and/or behaviourally recall the event after a 12- to 22-month delay. Children originally participated in the event either one or three times and experienced different parts of the event either at three distinct locations (spatial condition) or at a single location (nonspatial condition). Results show very little evidence of long-term memory for the event after one to two years. Returning children did not verbally recall the event, and they did not perform more actions or sequence the event more accurately than controls, with the exception of the older experimental children who had a tendency to sequence the event more accurately than same-aged controls. Although the results indicate that young children's memory for novel events is not very enduring, there were individual differences in children's ability to remember the event. These differences are discussed in terms of potential differences in cognitive abilities and changing knowledge about retrieval strategies or memory. 相似文献
157.
Marvin W. Kahn 《Journal of personality assessment》2013,95(4):44-47
Reality adherence measured by Mayman's system of Rorschach form level scoring was applied to the protocols of 43 individuals all of whom were pleading insanity to a charge of murder. Reality adherence level correlated significantly with judgment of legal sanity, contributed importantly to a factor analysis dimension of reality functioning, and was extensively related to measures of primary process thought and impulse. Reality adherence further was related to intellectual functioning and areas of past history. These results suggest that Mayman's reality adherence measure has substantial validity and is importantly related to other personality variables. 相似文献
158.
Ibrahim A. Kira Linda Lewandowski Jeffrey S. Ashby Cheryl Somers Lisa Chiodo Lydia Odenat 《Journal of aggression, maltreatment & trauma》2013,22(5):431-453
This study explored the effects of bullying victimization (BV), as a Type II traumatic stressor, on intelligence quotient (IQ), posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and complex PTSD symptoms. Participants were 390 African American and Iraqi refugee adolescents. Measures of BV, cumulative life trauma, PTSD, discrimination, and IQ were administered. Correlational, multilevel regression, and path analyses were conducted. BV, independent of cumulative trauma, and discrimination were found to have significant direct effects on increased PTSD symptoms and significant direct and indirect negative effects on perceptual reasoning, processing speed, and working memory. BV was also associated with increases in the discrepancy between perceptual reasoning and verbal comprehension. The implications of these results for the prevention and treatment of bullying victims are discussed. 相似文献
159.
Delay discounting occurs when the subjective value of an outcome decreases because its delivery is delayed. Previous research has suggested that the rate at which some, but not all, outcomes are discounted varies as a function of regular church attendance. In the present study, 509 participants completed measures of intrinsic religiousness, extrinsic religiousness, religious fundamentalism, and whether they regularly attended church services. They then completed a delay-discounting task involving five outcomes. Although religiousness was not a significant predictor of discounting for all outcomes, participants scoring high in intrinsic religiousness tended to display less delay discounting than participants scoring low. Likewise, participants scoring high in religious fundamentalism tended to display more delay discounting than participants scoring low. These results partially replicate previous ones in showing that the process of discounting may vary as a function of religiousness. The results also provide some direction for those interested in altering how individuals discount. 相似文献
160.
Mark A. Barnett Jeffrey S. Bartel Susan R. Burns Fred W. Sanborn Neal E. Christensen Molly M. White 《The Journal of genetic psychology》2013,174(3):381-383
KILMARTIN (1994) suggested that masculine traits may result from externalizing defense style and that when unconscious and unwanted feminine traits about the self occur, they are dealt with in an external way, radier man being directiy resolved by the individual. However, this dieory has received little empirical support. Lobel and Winch (1986) measured defense style among male college students; they found mat men who scored higher in masculinity tended to score higher on acting out (finding an object to attack) and splitting (separating affect from content). Lobel and Winch concluded mat use of diese defense styles reflects avoidance of emotional expression. In turn, diese findings have been supported by research that has suggested mat men use avoidance coping strategies more man women do (McCall &; Struthers, 1994). 相似文献