全文获取类型
收费全文 | 8619篇 |
免费 | 120篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
8740篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 77篇 |
2019年 | 100篇 |
2018年 | 170篇 |
2017年 | 161篇 |
2016年 | 174篇 |
2015年 | 116篇 |
2014年 | 141篇 |
2013年 | 705篇 |
2012年 | 280篇 |
2011年 | 282篇 |
2010年 | 164篇 |
2009年 | 178篇 |
2008年 | 266篇 |
2007年 | 285篇 |
2006年 | 259篇 |
2005年 | 227篇 |
2004年 | 218篇 |
2003年 | 211篇 |
2002年 | 214篇 |
2001年 | 256篇 |
2000年 | 250篇 |
1999年 | 169篇 |
1998年 | 94篇 |
1997年 | 87篇 |
1996年 | 95篇 |
1995年 | 73篇 |
1992年 | 139篇 |
1991年 | 141篇 |
1990年 | 133篇 |
1989年 | 123篇 |
1988年 | 117篇 |
1987年 | 116篇 |
1986年 | 123篇 |
1985年 | 131篇 |
1984年 | 125篇 |
1983年 | 95篇 |
1979年 | 112篇 |
1978年 | 86篇 |
1977年 | 78篇 |
1976年 | 79篇 |
1975年 | 77篇 |
1974年 | 107篇 |
1973年 | 113篇 |
1972年 | 76篇 |
1971年 | 74篇 |
1970年 | 82篇 |
1969年 | 76篇 |
1968年 | 109篇 |
1967年 | 83篇 |
1966年 | 94篇 |
排序方式: 共有8740条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
161.
162.
Joel Brockner Jeffrey Z Rubin Judy Fine Thomas P Hamilton Barbara Thomas Beth Turetsky 《Journal of research in personality》1982,16(2):247-266
Entrapping conflicts are those in which individuals continue to invest their resources—even in seemingly losing propositions-in large part to justify previous unfilled expenditures. It has been demonstrated that individuals define their motivation for investing very differently at the various stages of an entrapping conflict. One implication of this motivational dynamic is that various factors may differentially affect degree of entrapment (i.e., amount invested), depending upon the point in time at which they are introduced. The present studies were designed to test this notion. All subjects were given an initial monetary stake and had the opportunity to win more by taking part in an entrapping investment situation. In Experiment 1, half the subjects were provided with a payoff chart that made salient the costs associated with investing (Highcost salience condition) whereas half were not (Low-cost salience condition). Moreover, for half of the subjects the payoff chart was introduced before they were asked to invest (Early condition) whereas for the other half it was introduced after they had invested a considerable portion of their resources (Late condition). Entrapment was lower in the High salience-Early than in the Low salience-Early condition. However, there was no difference between groups in the Late condition. In Experiment 2, the perceived presence of an audience interacted with personality variables related to face-saving to effect entrapment. When the audience was described as “experts in decision making,” subjects high in public self-consciousness (or social anxiety) became less entrapped than those low on these dimensions. When the audience consisted of individuals who “wished simply to observe the experimental procedure,” however, high public self-consciousness (or social anxiety) individuals were significantly more entrapped than lows. Moreover, these interaction effects occurred when the audience was introduced late, but not early, into the entrapment situation. Taken together, these (and other) findings suggest that economic factors are more influential determinants of behavior in the earlier stages of an entrapping conflict, whereas face-saving variables are more potent in the later phases. Alternative explanations are discussed. 相似文献
163.
164.
Winett RA Hatcher JW Fort TR Leckliter IN Love SQ Riley AW Fishback JF 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1982,15(3):381-402
Two studies were conducted in all-electric townhouses and apartments in the winter (N = 83) and summer (N = 54) to ascertain how energy conservation strategies focusing on thermostat change and set-backs and other low-cost/no-cost approaches would affect overall electricity use and electricity used for heating and cooling, the home thermal environment, the perceived comfort of participants, and clothing that was worn. The studies assessed the effectiveness of videotape modeling programs that demonstrated these conservation strategies when used alone or combined with daily feedback on electricity use. In the winter, the results indicated that videotape modeling and/or feedback were effective relative to baseline and to a control group in reducing overall electricity use by about 15% and electricity used for heating by about 25%. Hygrothermographs, which accurately and continuously recorded temperature and humidity in the homes, indicated that participants were able to live with no reported loss in comfort and no change in attire at a mean temperature of about 62 degrees F when home and about 59 degrees F when asleep. The results were highly discrepant with prior laboratory studies indicating comfort at 75 degrees F with the insulation value of the clothing worn by participants in this study. In the summer, a combination of strategies designed to keep a home cool with minimal or no air conditioning, in conjunction with videotape modeling and/or daily feedback, resulted in overall electricity reductions of about 15% with reductions on electricity for cooling of about 34%, but with feedback, and feedback and modeling more effective than modeling alone. Despite these electricity savings, hygrothermograph recordings indicated minimal temperature change in the homes, with no change in perceived comfort or clothing worn. The results are discussed in terms of discrepancies with laboratory studies, optimal combinations of video-media and personal contact to promote behavior change, and energy policies that may be mislabeled as sacrificial and underestimate the effectiveness of conservation strategies such as those investigated in these studies. 相似文献
165.
The possibility of isolating personality types using 16 PF profiles was studied by utilizing a Q-type factor analytic procedure. Four modal profiles, characteristic of major clusters of subjects, were derived and cross validated. These four major types successfully classified approximately 2/3 of the 16 PF protocols (n = 1037), and classified similar proportions of previously published profiles. Variation of classification rates as a function of race and sex was described, and strategies for utilizing the profiles for research and interpretive purposes were outlined. 相似文献
166.
Stephen E. Newstead Kenneth I. Manktelow Jonathan St B. T. Evans 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》1982,2(1-3):21-32
Two experiments were carried out to investigate whether visual imagery was used in representing transitive linear ordering relationships. Subjects were presented with passages describing either a linear ordering or a set inclusion relationship, while being subjected to either visual or verbal interference. Performance was tested by asking subjects to judge the truth or falsity of statements concerning both the information presented in the passages and inferences that could be drawn from this information. In neither experiment was there any evidence for the linear ordering material being selectively disrupted by the visual interference task, as would have been predicted by the imagery theory. Thus it is concluded that linear orderings are probably not represented as visual images. 相似文献
167.
168.
The present research investigated the effects of experimentally induced positive and negative feedback about the self on the recipient's subsequent reactions to aid. In line with a consistency prediction, subjects given positive feedback who later received aid had lower mood ratings and less favorable self-evaluations than those who were given positive feedback but no subsequent help. Conversely, subjects given negative feedback who later received aid had higher mood ratings and more positive self-evaluations than those who were given negative feedback but no subsequent help. Subjects' evaluations of another individual were characterized by aid main effects, which indicate that an other who helped was evaluated more favorably than an other who did not help. The conceptual and applied implications of these results are discussed. 相似文献
169.
170.