全文获取类型
收费全文 | 710篇 |
免费 | 11篇 |
专业分类
721篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 1篇 |
2022年 | 6篇 |
2021年 | 2篇 |
2020年 | 10篇 |
2019年 | 12篇 |
2018年 | 19篇 |
2017年 | 23篇 |
2016年 | 36篇 |
2015年 | 18篇 |
2014年 | 24篇 |
2013年 | 49篇 |
2012年 | 34篇 |
2011年 | 50篇 |
2010年 | 25篇 |
2009年 | 39篇 |
2008年 | 46篇 |
2007年 | 40篇 |
2006年 | 45篇 |
2005年 | 22篇 |
2004年 | 32篇 |
2003年 | 26篇 |
2002年 | 12篇 |
2001年 | 12篇 |
2000年 | 8篇 |
1999年 | 6篇 |
1998年 | 12篇 |
1997年 | 7篇 |
1996年 | 11篇 |
1995年 | 12篇 |
1994年 | 11篇 |
1993年 | 6篇 |
1992年 | 5篇 |
1991年 | 6篇 |
1990年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 5篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1982年 | 4篇 |
1981年 | 7篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 7篇 |
1977年 | 7篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1972年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有721条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
281.
Greenberg J Martens A Jonas E Eisenstadt D Pyszczynski T Solomon S 《Psychological science》2003,14(5):516-519
A large body of research has shown that when people are reminded of their mortality, their defense of their cultural worldview intensifies. Although some psychological defenses seem to be instigated by negative affective responses to threat, mortality salience does not appear to arouse such affect. Terror management theory posits that the potential to experience anxiety, rather than the actual experience of anxiety, underlies these effects of mortality salience. If this is correct, then mortality-salience effects should be reduced when participants believe they are not capable of reacting to the reminder of mortality with anxiety. In a test of this hypothesis, participants consumed a placebo purported to either block anxiety or enhance memory. Then we manipulated mortality salience, and participants evaluated pro- and anti-American essays as a measure of worldview defense. Although mortality salience intensified worldview defense in the memory-enhancer condition, this effect was completely eliminated in the anxiety-blocker condition. The results suggest that some psychological defenses serve to avert the experience of anxiety rather than to ameliorate actually experienced anxiety. 相似文献
282.
Enrica Ciucci Andrea Baroncelli Giovanna Tambasco Jeff Laurent Salvatore J. Catanzaro Thomas E. JoinerJr. 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2017,39(3):373-382
Italian-language versions of the Positive and Negative Affect Scale for Children (PANAS-C; Laurent et al. Psychological Assessment, 11, 326–338, 1999) and Physiological Hyperarousal Scale for Children (PH-C; Laurent et al. Psychological Assessment, 16, 373–380, 2004) were developed. Students (N = 1026; M age = 12.05 years, SD = 1.49) from central Italy completed these scales. Measures were evaluated based on criteria from previous studies resulting in an 11-item PANAS-C PA scale (α = .88), a 13-item PANAS-C NA scale (α = .87), and a 14-item PH-C (α = .85). Confirmatory factor analyses resulted in best-fit indices that favored a 3-factor model consistent with the tripartite model of anxiety and depression. Similar to results reported with the original English-language and other translations of these measures, scores on the PA scale were lower and scores on the NA and PH scales were higher for older girls than older boys and younger boys and girls. Findings suggested that the psychometric properties of the Italian-language versions of the PANAS-C and PH-C were similar to the English-language versions and other translated versions of the measures. 相似文献
283.
Jeffrey Yoshimi 《Husserl Studies》2009,25(2):121-140
I develop a “two-systems” interpretation of Husserl’s theory of belief. On this interpretation, Husserl accounts for our sense
of the world in terms of (1) a system of embodied horizon meanings and passive synthesis, which is involved in any experience
of an object, and (2) a system of active synthesis and sedimentation, which comes on line when we attend to an object’s properties.
I use this account to defend Husserl against several forms of Heideggerean critique. One line of critique, recently elaborated
by Taylor Carman, says that Husserl wrongly loads everyday perception with explicit beliefs about things. A second, earlier
line of critique, due to Hubert Dreyfus, charges Husserl with thinking of belief on a problematic Artificial Intelligence
(AI) model which involves explicit rules applied to discrete symbol structures. I argue that these criticisms are based on
a conflation of Husserl’s two systems of belief. The conception of Husserlian phenomenology which emerges is compatible with
Heideggerean phenomenology and associated approaches to cognitive science (in particular, dynamical systems theory).
相似文献
Jeffrey YoshimiEmail: Email: |
284.
285.
286.
Arndt J Greenberg J Schimel J Pyszczynski T Solomon S 《Journal of personality and social psychology》2002,83(1):26-43
The terror management prediction that reminders of death motivate in-group identification assumes people view their identifications positively. However, when the in-group is framed negatively, mortality salience should lead to disidentification. Study 1 found that mortality salience increased women's perceived similarity to other women except under gender-based stereotype threat. In Study 2, mortality salience and a negative ethnic prime led Hispanic as well as Anglo participants to derogate paintings attributed to Hispanic (but not Anglo-American) artists. Study 3 added a neutral prime condition and used a more direct measure of psychological distancing. Mortality salience and the negative prime led Hispanic participants to view themselves as especially different from a fellow Hispanic. Implications for understanding in-group derogation and disidentification are briefly discussed. 相似文献
287.
Why do women underperform under stereotype threat? Evidence for the role of negative thinking 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
This study investigated the role of negative thinking as a potential mediator of performance deficits under stereotype threat. After being assigned to a stereotype-threat or a no-threat condition, 60 female participants were asked to complete a difficult math task. Using the thought-listing technique, women under stereotype threat reported a higher number of negative thoughts specifically related to the test and to mathematics compared with women in the no-threat condition. Moreover, women under stereotype threat also showed a sharp decrease in performance that (a) was most pronounced in the second half of the test and (b) was mediated by the increase in negative thinking. 相似文献
288.
Arndt J Routledge C Greenberg J Sheldon KM 《Personality & social psychology bulletin》2005,31(10):1327-1339
Previous research indicates that mortality salience and creative behavior combine to increase feelings of guilt, presumably over the disruption to social connection elicited by the call for innovative expression. The present studies examined whether satiating assimilation motives by highlighting conformity to others reduces this effect (Study 1) and facilitates positive psychological engagement (Study 2). Study 1 used a 2 (conformity vs. neutral feedback)x2 (mortality salience vs. control)x2 (creative task vs. noncreative task) design and had participants complete a self-report measure of guilt. Study 2 used a 2 (mortality salience vs. control)x2 (other goal task vs. self-goal task) design, and after a creativity exercise, had participants complete measures of positive mood, vitality, and creative problem solving. Results indicated attending to assimilation needs reduced the elevated guilt that follows the juxtaposition of mortality salience and creative behavior and also increased a sense of positive engagement. Implications are briefly discussed. 相似文献
289.
Cox CR Arndt J Pyszczynski T Greenberg J Abdollahi A Solomon S 《Journal of personality and social psychology》2008,94(4):696-717
Six studies examined the role of young adults' parental attachment in terror management. Studies 1-3 revealed that activating thoughts of one's parent in response to mortality salience (MS) reduced death-thought accessibility and worldview defense and increased feelings of self-worth. Studies 4-5 demonstrated that MS led to greater ease of recalling positive maternal interactions and greater difficulty recalling negative interactions, and increased attraction to a stranger who was described as being similar to one's parent. If reliance on parents for terror management purposes reflects the operation of attachment mechanisms, then such effects should vary on the basis of an individual's attachment style. Study 6 demonstrated that, after MS, insecure individuals were more likely to rely on relationships with their parents, whereas secure individuals were more likely to rely on relationships with romantic partners. 相似文献
290.
According to terror management theory, the annihilation of people who threaten one's worldview should serve the function of defending that worldview. The present research assessed this hypothesis. A sample of Christian participants read either a worldview-threatening news article reporting on the Muslimization of Nazareth or a nonthreatening article about the aurora borealis. Half of the participants in the worldview-threat condition were informed at the end of the article that a number of Muslims had died in a plane crash on their way to Nazareth. Although reading the threatening news article increased death-thought accessibility and worldview defense relative to reading the neutral article, these increases were not observed among participants who learned that a number of Muslims were dead. Implications for understanding protracted intergroup conflict are discussed. 相似文献