全文获取类型
收费全文 | 674篇 |
免费 | 33篇 |
专业分类
707篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 1篇 |
2022年 | 6篇 |
2021年 | 2篇 |
2020年 | 10篇 |
2019年 | 11篇 |
2018年 | 19篇 |
2017年 | 23篇 |
2016年 | 36篇 |
2015年 | 18篇 |
2014年 | 24篇 |
2013年 | 47篇 |
2012年 | 33篇 |
2011年 | 49篇 |
2010年 | 25篇 |
2009年 | 36篇 |
2008年 | 46篇 |
2007年 | 39篇 |
2006年 | 45篇 |
2005年 | 22篇 |
2004年 | 31篇 |
2003年 | 26篇 |
2002年 | 12篇 |
2001年 | 12篇 |
2000年 | 8篇 |
1999年 | 6篇 |
1998年 | 12篇 |
1997年 | 7篇 |
1996年 | 11篇 |
1995年 | 12篇 |
1994年 | 11篇 |
1993年 | 6篇 |
1992年 | 5篇 |
1991年 | 6篇 |
1990年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 5篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1982年 | 4篇 |
1981年 | 7篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 4篇 |
1977年 | 7篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1972年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有707条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
Pyszczynski T Abdollahi A Solomon S Greenberg J Cohen F Weise D 《Personality & social psychology bulletin》2006,32(4):525-537
Study 1 investigated the effect of mortality salience on support for martyrdom attacks among Iranian college students. Participants were randomly assigned to answer questions about either their own death or an aversive topic unrelated to death and then evaluated materials from fellow students who either supported or opposed martyrdom attacks against the United States. Whereas control participants preferred the student who opposed martyrdom, participants reminded of death preferred the student who supported martyrdom and indicated they were more likely to consider such activities themselves. Study 2 investigated the effect of mortality salience on American college students' support for extreme military interventions by American forces that could kill thousands of civilians. Mortality salience increased support for such measures among politically conservative but not politically liberal students. The roles of existential fear, cultural worldviews, and construing one's nation as pursing a heroic battle against evil in advocacy of violence were discussed. 相似文献
72.
A simple reaction time (RT) experiment was conducted to test the hypothesis that redundancy gain arises partly because of hemispheric coactivation. Stimuli were presented to the left or right of fixation, or redundantly to both, and the participants had to make keypress responses as rapidly as possible to stimulus onset. A "static" condition, in which the participants held their hands at rest, was compared with a "dynamic" condition, in which they moved their hands back and forth in an oscillating motion prior to stimulus onset. As predicted from the hypothesis of hemispheric coactivation, redundancy gain, measured as the decrease in mean RT to redundant stimuli as compared with single stimuli, was smaller in the dynamic condition than in the static one. 相似文献
73.
When people must respond discriminatively to 1 or 2 stimuli by making 1 or 2 taps of a response key, they initiate the response more rapidly when the correct number of taps matches the number of stimuli (compatible condition) than when it mismatches (incompatible condition; J. O. Miller, S. G. Atkins, & F. Van Nes, 2005). Miller et al. sometimes found an effect of compatibility on response execution time, as reflected in the interresponse intervals between successive taps. The authors report 2 further experiments (N = 8 participants) in which they generalized the numerosity compatibility effects on response-initiation time and interresponse intervals to 2- versus 3-stimulus sequences. In addition, they varied gap length between stimuli to see whether the rhythm of the stimulus would influence that of the response. Weak rhythmicity effects were repeatedly found, but those were too small to suggest a plausible alternative explanation for the numerosity compatibility effect on response-initiation time. 相似文献
74.
75.
Correlation Weights in Multiple Regression 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
A general theory on the use of correlation weights in linear prediction has yet to be proposed. In this paper we take initial
steps in developing such a theory by describing the conditions under which correlation weights perform well in population
regression models. Using OLS weights as a comparison, we define cases in which the two weighting systems yield maximally correlated
composites and when they yield minimally similar weights. We then derive the least squares weights (for any set of predictors)
that yield the largest drop in R
2 (the coefficient of determination) when switching to correlation weights. Our findings suggest that two characteristics of
a model/data combination are especially important in determining the effectiveness of correlation weights: (1) the condition
number of the predictor correlation matrix, R
xx
, and (2) the orientation of the correlation weights to the latent vectors of R
xx
. 相似文献
76.
Jeff S. Johnson 《Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management》2013,33(3):262-273
The dominant research paradigm in sales research involves testing theory through empirical research. Nascent or underdeveloped research areas, however, may lack or have inadequate existing theories to explain sales-related phenomena. In these cases, sales researchers require a theory-generating methodological approach. Qualitative research designs are useful in this pursuit. The purpose of this article is to provide an exposition of one such qualitative research design – grounded theory. To this end, the foundational processes of grounded theory methodology are discussed. The results of a review of grounded theory examinations conducted in sales research are also provided, and current practices utilized by sales grounded theorists are discussed. Based on this review, future directions in substantive areas and methodological practices are provided. This article aims to serve as a resource for sales scholars wishing to know what grounded theory examinations have been conducted, how to implement grounded theory research and what avenues are available for future grounded theory sales research. 相似文献
77.
Event-related potentials (ERPs) were obtained from 15 electrode sites in six average and six impaired reading children, 12 years of age, during visual letter discrimination tasks. Subjects responded to target letters with an enclosed area in the form task and to letters that rhymed with “e” in the rhyme task. Response accuracy was similar between the groups. Reaction time was relatively longer for the impaired group during the rhyme task. At lateral sites, condition differences were evident as greater negative shifts in the rhyme task than the form at 170 and 470 ms as well as a delayed late positivity for the rhyme. In terms of reading ability, the average readers’ ERPs were more negative than those of the impaired group at 270 and 450 ms. Interhemispheric variations were also seen between the groups, with the average readers more negative than the impaired readers at right hemisphere sites. Contrary to expectations, group differences in the ERP did not vary substantially as a function of condition, and task demands were evaluated in view of these findings. 相似文献
78.
Jeff Astley 《Mental health, religion & culture》2013,16(9):975-978
This brief piece relates Edward Bailey's concept of implicit religion, together with some themes from the papers published in this special issue, to Astley's concept of ordinary theology. In particular, it attends to their different, but overlapping, focal concerns and their shared emphasis on a spiritual core of personal meaningfulness. 相似文献
79.
Itiel E. Dror Saul M. Kassin Jeff Kukucka 《Journal of applied research in memory and cognition》2013,2(1):78-81
Psychology has made a tremendous contribution to law by showing the malleability of eyewitness perception and memory, and developing best practices for obtaining eyewitness identifications. We suggest that even expert scientific witnesses, which the court heavily relies on as objective and impartial, are also susceptible to bias from various psychological influences. For example, forensic examiners’ interactions with detectives and exposure to information about the case can bias their judgments. We discuss the ten commentaries on these issues across a range of forensic science domains, and affirm what reforms are needed. 相似文献
80.
When a line is presented in the vicinity of a recent luminance change (peripheral cue), it is perceived to be drawn over time away from its “cued” end even though the line is actually presented all at once. This study was designed to determine whether attention, exogenously attracted to the cue, would come under the exogenous control of this illusory motion and follow the drawing motion from the cued end to its terminus. Each trial began with the display of four small squares at the corners of an imaginary square centered about fixation. On the critical trials, one of the four squares brightened briefly, after which a horizontal line was presented joining either the two upper or the two lower squares. Shortly thereafter, the distribution of attention was determined by asking the observer to indicate the nature of a change that was equally likely to occur to one of the squares. Responses to targets presented at a noncued location that was at the end of an illusorily drawn line were as fast as those to targets at the cued location and were much faster than those to targets at the remaining noncued locations. This pattern was not shown when the line preceded the cue, strongly suggesting that attention follows the motion in this illusion. 相似文献