首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2187篇
  免费   121篇
  2023年   9篇
  2022年   20篇
  2021年   19篇
  2020年   44篇
  2019年   58篇
  2018年   89篇
  2017年   90篇
  2016年   101篇
  2015年   51篇
  2014年   86篇
  2013年   199篇
  2012年   117篇
  2011年   127篇
  2010年   69篇
  2009年   84篇
  2008年   113篇
  2007年   113篇
  2006年   93篇
  2005年   64篇
  2004年   79篇
  2003年   80篇
  2002年   73篇
  2001年   44篇
  2000年   36篇
  1999年   27篇
  1998年   37篇
  1997年   20篇
  1996年   30篇
  1995年   28篇
  1994年   29篇
  1993年   14篇
  1992年   19篇
  1991年   18篇
  1990年   15篇
  1989年   13篇
  1988年   19篇
  1987年   11篇
  1986年   12篇
  1985年   14篇
  1984年   10篇
  1983年   9篇
  1982年   12篇
  1981年   13篇
  1980年   11篇
  1979年   9篇
  1978年   16篇
  1977年   11篇
  1976年   10篇
  1972年   6篇
  1969年   6篇
排序方式: 共有2308条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
931.
A variety of procedures have been used to promote spontaneous requesting in persons with developmental disabilities. However, effective use of such procedures depends in part on identifying the stimuli that gained control of requests during intervention. In the present study, two adults with severe disabilities were taught to request a preferred food item with a modified gesture for “eat.” After learning to respond when the trainer held up an item, an assessment of stimulus control was conducted to determine if requests would occur spontaneously, that is, without the trainer having to offer the item. One learner displayed a number of spontaneous requests during the assessment. Natural maintaining contingencies were then introduced to maintain his spontaneous requests. The other learner made no spontaneous requests during the initial assessment, but subsequent tests revealed that he sometimes requested as the trainer was about to display the item. It thus appeared that both learners were responding at an earlier point in the trial sequence. The results suggest that an assessment of stimulus control may be helpful when selecting strategies to promote spontaneous requesting.  相似文献   
932.
The present study tests the theories of reasoned action and planned behavior in the context of women's participation in collective action. Participants were 387 women who completed two questionnaires with an interval of 1 year. In the first questionnaire, participants were asked to provide measures of attitudinal, normative, and control factors, as well as measures of intention to participate in 4 group-related activities over the coming year. One year later in the second questionnaire, the same women provided measures of the degree to which they had actually participated in these behaviors during the course of the year. Findings showed strong relationships between attitudinal factors and intentions, and between intentions and behavior. The addition of perceived behavioral control, as specified by the theory of planned behavior, made little difference in regression analyses. Further analyses focused on the mediating role of identification as an activist. This revealed that attitudinal considerations were only important in the prediction of intention for those individuals with a weaker sense of themselves as activists. It is suggested that expectancy-value calculations may be less important in predicting the intentions of strongly committed individuals to engage in group-relevant acts.  相似文献   
933.
Previous research has shown that reminding subjects of their mortality encourages negative reactions to others whose behaviour or attitudes deviate from the cultural worldview (e.g. Greenberg, Pyszczynski, Solomon, Rosenblatt, Veeder, Kirkland and Lyon 1990; Greenberg, Simon, Pyszczynski, Solomon and Chatel 1992; Rosenblatt, Greenberg, Solomon, Pyszczynski and Lyon 1989). According to terror management theory, these findings result from a heightened need for faith in the cultural worldview that is activated by reminders of one's mortality. Study I assessed the plausibility of an alternative explanation which posits that mortality salience simply primes individuals' values. Whereas mortality salience led to harsher bond recommendations for a prostitute, a procedure that directly focused subjects on their values did not. Studies 2 and 3 assessed the possibility that reminding subjects of any worrisome future concern would produce the same effect as a reminder of mortality. In both studies, mortality salience led to negative reactions to a deviant and had no effect on self-reported affect, whereas other worrisome thoughts had no effect on reactions to a deviant but did create negative affect. Thus, consistent with terror management theory, mortality salience effects seem to result exclusively from thoughts of death.  相似文献   
934.
This qualitative pilot study was designed to identify and explain significant events for patients participating in a psychodynamic psychotherapy group for eating disorders. Specifically, seven members of a mixed (i.e., anorectic, bulimic, obese) eating disorders group recorded what they perceived as the three most significant events in group meetings for 14 weeks. In addition, group members were instructed to record why each event was significant. Manifest and latent content analyses of the data revealed that members found feedback and observing others the two most common types of significant events, and emotional experience, insight, and relationship the reasons these events had such impact. The implications of these results for working with the eating-disordered patient in group, as well as their implication for general group theory and practice, are discussed.An earlier vesion of this paper was presented at the 1991 meeting of the American Group Psychotherapy Association, New York. This paper was funded, in part, by a California State Faculty Support Grant and a Santa Barbara Cottage Hospital Research Grant awarded to the first author. The authors would like to thank Sean T. Casey, Sally Barton, Dr. Allison Wollitzer, Tom Knowlton, Craig Park, and Beth Buxton for their assistance in the preparation and execution of this study.  相似文献   
935.
936.
This article reviews the essential features, types, prevalence, pathophysiology, and neuropsychological correlates associated with the sleep apnea syndrome. Persons who experience the intermittent hypoxia and fragmented sleep characteristic of the sleep apnea syndrome tend to exhibit moderate symptoms of diffuse cognitive dysfunction as well as multiple emotional and psychosocial sequela. It is concluded that more research is required in order to elucidate the relationship between the hypoxic parameters and neurocognitive deficits seen in the sleep apnea syndrome, and that neuropsychological assessment might represent a means whereby the effectiveness of various treatments for sleep apnea may be evaluated.  相似文献   
937.
938.
When a visual display contains two targets, both of which require the same response, reaction times (RTs) are faster than when only one target appears. This effect has previously been obtained regardless of whether the redundant targets are the same or different in shape, and in at least one set of two-target experiments, the redundancy gains have been larger for different targets (Grice & Reed, 1992). Experiments with two different targets have also revealed violations of the race-model inequality, suggesting that redundant targets coactivate the response (Miller, 1982). The present paper reexamines both of these findings, because both appear to be inconsistent with the interactive race model (Mordkoff & Yantis, 1991). Experiment 1 shows that the race-model inequality is not violated when the experimental design is free of biased contingencies; Experiment 1 also provides evidence that target preferences may artifactually produce the RT advantage fordifferent- oversame-target trials. Experiment 2, however, shows that the race-model inequality is violated when the frequencies of single- and redundant-target displays are equated (without introducing any biased contingencies), implying that the interactive race model cannot account for the results of experiments involving more than one type of target. Alternative loci for coactivation are briefly discussed.  相似文献   
939.
940.
The career preferences of a cohort of 1,773 pupils in ten co-educational comprehensive schools were studied at three points between the ages of 11 and 17. At all three ages, there were enormous differences between the sexes in their job plans, although girls and boys gave similar responses to questions about the characteristics of their ideal job. There were also significant differences between children of different social class and academic ability, and some indications of differences between ethnic groups. Individual children showed little consistency in the occupations they named, although some jobs, notably hairdressing and teaching, were chosen at an earlier age than others. One of the strongest predictors of the occupational aspirations of 17 year-olds was their perception of what their parents wanted for them when they were 14. Boys' aspirations seemed to be based on their individual abilities and achievements, whereas girls' aspirations were more strongly related to ascribed factors such as social class.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号