全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1014篇 |
免费 | 12篇 |
专业分类
1026篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 9篇 |
2022年 | 9篇 |
2021年 | 10篇 |
2020年 | 18篇 |
2019年 | 22篇 |
2018年 | 38篇 |
2017年 | 28篇 |
2016年 | 37篇 |
2015年 | 24篇 |
2014年 | 18篇 |
2013年 | 95篇 |
2012年 | 36篇 |
2011年 | 40篇 |
2010年 | 26篇 |
2009年 | 36篇 |
2008年 | 44篇 |
2007年 | 35篇 |
2006年 | 34篇 |
2005年 | 26篇 |
2004年 | 31篇 |
2003年 | 16篇 |
2002年 | 23篇 |
2001年 | 24篇 |
2000年 | 24篇 |
1999年 | 16篇 |
1998年 | 11篇 |
1997年 | 11篇 |
1996年 | 11篇 |
1995年 | 9篇 |
1994年 | 8篇 |
1992年 | 19篇 |
1991年 | 14篇 |
1990年 | 10篇 |
1989年 | 16篇 |
1988年 | 10篇 |
1987年 | 8篇 |
1986年 | 9篇 |
1985年 | 19篇 |
1984年 | 11篇 |
1983年 | 7篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1981年 | 9篇 |
1977年 | 5篇 |
1975年 | 8篇 |
1974年 | 10篇 |
1972年 | 8篇 |
1971年 | 6篇 |
1968年 | 14篇 |
1967年 | 9篇 |
1965年 | 5篇 |
排序方式: 共有1026条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
821.
C R Fletcher J E Hummel C J Marsolek 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》1990,16(2):233-240
Recent research has suggested that each statement in a narrative text is understood by relating it to its causal antecedents and consequences and that the text as a whole is understood by finding a causal path linking its opening to its final outcome. Fletcher and Bloom (1988) have proposed that in order to accomplish this goal, while minimizing the number of times long-term memory has to be searched, readers focus their attention on the last clause of a narrative that has causal antecedents but no consequences in the preceding text. As a result, a statement that is followed by a causal antecedent should remain the focus of attention, while the same statement followed by a consequence should not. This prediction was tested and confirmed in three experiments which show that when a target statement is followed by a sentence that includes only causal antecedents, (a) continuation sentences related to it are read more quickly, (b) target words drawn from it are easier to recognize, and (c) subject-generated continuations are more likely to be causally related to it. 相似文献
822.
Christopher S. Hill 《Philosophical Studies》2016,173(3):827-829
823.
This work was designed to study changes in neonatal behavior during the first two days of life. Infants in Part I were from middle-class backgrounds. Half of these infants had unrestricted maternal contact during the first two days of life, the other half had contact with their mothers only at feeding time. Infants in Part II were from low socio-economic backgrounds. They were cared for entirely by the nursery staff during the first two days of life, so that differences in maternal style did not affect their behavior. Half of these babies were born to mothers who received 50 mg meperidine during labor, half to mothers who received no meperidine. This work provided evidence for the sensitivity of the neonate during the first two days of life. Both those infants who received unrestricted maternal contact and those whose mothers had received no obstetrical drugs exhibited more behavioral change than those who received low maternal contact or whose mothers had received meperidine during labor. High maternal contact and absence of obstetrical drugs also resulted in increased sensitivity to some environmental stimuli. 相似文献
824.
Vaughn S Cirino PT Wanzek J Wexler J Fletcher JM Denton CD Barth A Romain M Francis DJ 《School psychology review》2010,39(1):3-21
This study examined the effectiveness of a yearlong, researcher-provided, Tier 2 (secondary) intervention with a group of sixth-graders. The intervention emphasized word recognition, vocabulary, fluency, and comprehension, Participants scored below a proficiency level on their slate accountability test and were compared to a similar group of struggling readers receiving school-provided instruction. All students received the benefits of content area teachers who participated in researcher-provided professional development designed to integrate vocabulary and comprehension practices throughout the school day (Tier 1). Students who participated in the Tier 2 intervention showed gains on measures of decoding, fluency, and comprehension, but differences relative to students in the comparison group were small (median d = +0.16). Students who received the re searcher-provided intervention scored significantly higher than students who received comparison intervention on measures of word attack, spelling, the state accountability measure, passage comprehension, and phonemic decoding efficiency, although most often in particular subgroups. 相似文献
825.
Experiments are reported of the nonvisual sensitivity of observers to their paths of locomotion and to the resulting changes in the structure of their perspectives, ie changes in the network of directions and distances spatially relating them to objects fixed in the surrounding environment. In the first experiment it was found that adults can keep up to date on the changing structure of their perspectives even in the absence of sights and sounds that specify changes in self-to-object relations. They do this rapidly, accurately, and, according to the subjects' reports, automatically, as if perceiving the new perspective structures. The second experiment was designed to investigate the role of visual experience in the development of sensitivity to occluded changes in perspective structure by comparing the judgments of sighted adults with those of late-blinded adults (who had extensive life histories of vision) and those of early-blinded adults (who had little or no history of vision). The three groups performed similarly when asked to judge perspective while imagining a new point of observation. However, locomoting to the new point greatly facilitated the judgments of the sighted and late-blinded subjects, but not those of the early-blinded subjects. The findings indicate that visual experience plays an important role in the development of sensitivity to changes in perspective structure when walking without vision. 相似文献
826.
827.
Jennifer Merrolla Zoe Nemerever Mackenzie Lockhart Thad Kousser Mindy Romero Seth Hill 《Political psychology》2023,44(5):1053-1076
How does the public react to information about the likely progression of COVID-19 cases in the United States? How do these reactions vary over the course of the pandemic and by partisanship, and with what consequences for policy attitudes and personal behavior? We argue that reading projections about the peak of COVID-19 cases in the United States is likely to lead to increased levels of anxiety and sadness. We expect that these effects will be more pronounced and less polarized along partisan lines earlier in the pandemic. Finally, we expect that elevated anxiety and sadness should in turn lead to greater support for protective policies to combat the pandemic and a greater inclination to engage in protective behaviors. To test these arguments, we fielded online survey experiments at three points in time (April, June, and August 2020), in which respondents were randomly assigned to a control group or one of two projections about the likely progression of COVID-19 cases in the United States. Across all three waves, we find that exposure to information about case peaks increases anxiety and sadness, though the effects get weaker over time, particularly among Republicans. We also find evidence that these elevated emotional responses increase support for protective policies and behavior. 相似文献
828.
829.
Activism is responsible for many of the positive social changes we have seen across time, and is central to pushing for continued social change. However, these positive outcomes do not come without a cost, as activism can also be draining and can lead to physical and emotional harm for those involved. Social policies and intervention programs have addressed how to help people maintain their engagement with activism aims, though few have considered the motivational role of purpose in life. As purpose in life involves a sustained commitment to one's goals, having a purpose in life centered around activism may help an individual remain committed to activism and to the creation of change in the face of these obstacles. If activist purpose can help us understand and predict who is likely to remain engaged in activism, then it is valuable to consider what factors influence the development of activist purpose, in order to continue necessary social change. Thus, in this work we define activist purpose, and theorize as to what factors may be relevant to the development of an activist purpose by situating predictors of activism and collective action within the literature on pathways to purpose in life. This work may thus provide a greater understanding of how to encourage activism or reduce the harmful effects associated with activism. 相似文献
830.