首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   1047篇
  免费   33篇
  2022年   7篇
  2021年   8篇
  2020年   18篇
  2019年   14篇
  2018年   28篇
  2017年   22篇
  2016年   28篇
  2015年   12篇
  2014年   32篇
  2013年   109篇
  2012年   43篇
  2011年   46篇
  2010年   23篇
  2009年   22篇
  2008年   43篇
  2007年   41篇
  2006年   35篇
  2005年   37篇
  2004年   34篇
  2003年   32篇
  2002年   44篇
  2001年   19篇
  2000年   20篇
  1999年   20篇
  1998年   16篇
  1997年   11篇
  1996年   19篇
  1995年   15篇
  1994年   12篇
  1992年   7篇
  1991年   12篇
  1990年   11篇
  1989年   14篇
  1988年   17篇
  1987年   12篇
  1986年   12篇
  1985年   14篇
  1984年   16篇
  1983年   13篇
  1982年   14篇
  1981年   7篇
  1979年   14篇
  1976年   7篇
  1975年   8篇
  1973年   11篇
  1972年   7篇
  1971年   7篇
  1969年   6篇
  1968年   8篇
  1967年   7篇
排序方式: 共有1080条查询结果,搜索用时 62 毫秒
31.
32.
33.
Ryan  Kathryn M.  Kanjorski  Jeanne 《Sex roles》1998,38(9-10):743-756
The current study tested Freud's (1905/1960)theory that sexist humor may be associated withhostility toward women and extended previous researchshowing a link between hostile humor and aggression.Colleges students (N = 399 — approximately 92%white, 5% African American, and 3% other minorities)rated 10 sexist jokes on their perceived funniness.Results showed that the enjoyment of sexist humor waspositively correlated with rape-related attitudes andbeliefs, the self-reported likelihood of forcing sex,and psychological, physical, and sexual aggression inmen. For women, the enjoyment of sexist humor was only positively correlated with Adversarial SexualBeliefs and Acceptance of Interpersonal Violence. Womenalso found the jokes to be less enjoyable, lessacceptable, and more offensive than the men, but they were not significantly less likely to tell thejokes.  相似文献   
34.
A variable interval (VI) schedule is described that automatically adjusts the programmed rates of reinforcement in accordance with the rates of responding of subjects during the two immediately preceding 30-sec time intervals. The schedule prescribes that as rate of responding decreases, programmed reinforcement rate increases, and that when rate of responding increases, reinforcement rate decreases. Thus, programmed reinforcement rate is adjusted continuously until some target value is reached. Ten rats were exposed to this procedure five times a day at 1-h intervals. The target, set at VI 120 sec, was reached by most subjects within 4 days of training. Subsequently, all subjects responded consistently during five daily 1-h sessions with VI 120 sec. This procedure speeds up the training of subjects on long VI schedules and substantially reduces the time and effort spent observing the subjects and adjusting the schedule parameter value during the early development of responding.  相似文献   
35.
Considerable scientific evidence demonstrates the reduction in risk for neural tube defects (NTDs) associated with maternal preconceptional folic acid supplementation. The National Society of Genetic Counselors (NSGC) endorses the U.S. Public Health Service recommendations for folic acid supplementation at the 0.4 mg level for women in the general population and at the 4.0 mg level for women at high or increased risk for NTD pregnancies for at least 4 weeks prior to active pursuit of conception. We encourage targeted educational efforts and surveillance to assess results of this dietary supplementation. The NSGC further urges the Food and Drug Administration to fortify staple foodstuffs with folic acid for a population-based approach to minimize the number of NTD births.  相似文献   
36.
37.
The influence of subject and interviewer characteristics on the reliability of self-reports of alcohol consumption among young adults was investigated. Data were gathered from black and white college students of both sexes (N's=24) using a time-line procedure. The results snowed that these young adults provided highly reliable self-reports regarding their use of alcohol. Test-retest correlations for a criterion interval of 90 days were .96, .93, and .97 for the numbers of abstinent, moderate-drinking, and heavy-drinking days, respectively. Analyses also showed that white female subjects generally provided more reliable reports of abstinent and heavy-drinking days and that white female interviewers gathered more reliable reports across the three drinking disposition categories. These findings suggest that nonalcoholic young adults' retrospective reports of their drinking behavior can be reliably assessed using the time-line methodology. Future research is required to determine the validity of these self-reports and to understand the differential influence of subject and interviewer characteristics on the levels of reliability found.  相似文献   
38.
Factor analyses were carried out on the responses of 115 cigarette smokers to the Smoking Questionnaire of Russell et al. (1974), to the Situational Smoking Questionnaire of Frith (1971) and to certain items of a biographical smoking questionnaire. In the analysis of Russell's questionnaire, a set of six factors was extracted, bearing some resemblance to the six factors found by the authors of the questionnaire, although the matching of factors was by no means perfect. However, the results did support Russell et al.'s finding of a major dimension of degree of dependence upon cigarettes, together with an orthogonal factor contrasting pharmacological with non-pharmacological dependence. The analysis of the Situational Smoking Questionnaire confirmed Frith's finding that the first principal component represented a general desire for cigarettes while the second contrasted desire to smoke in high- and low-arousal inducing (or stressful and relaxing) situations; and these two components corresponded fairly well with the first two components extracted from Russell's questionnaire, pharmacological dependence being associated with smoking in stressful situations.  相似文献   
39.
40.
Directional judgments are typically slower when relative location is described by the words “east” and “west” or “right” and “left” than when described by the words “north” and “south” or “up” and “down.” A series of experiments are reported that disentangle verbal from perceptual encoding explanations for right-left difficulty. Overall, our results support a verbal encoding explanation for right-left confusion in the adult. Experiments 1-3 demonstrate that in a response-differentiation task, it is response to the labels “north,” “east,” “south,” and “west” that is responsible for right-left confusion. In addition, Experiments 4-6 demonstrate that right-left difficulty in a mirror image discrimination task is contingent on the use of directional labels. (The data also suggest that it may be more difficult to deal with “up,” “down,” “left,” and “right” than with “north,” “south,” “east,” and “west”) The data are interpreted as inconsistent with a bilateral symmetry explanation for right-left confusion.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号