首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   40599篇
  免费   1800篇
  国内免费   15篇
  42414篇
  2020年   442篇
  2019年   441篇
  2018年   600篇
  2017年   669篇
  2016年   741篇
  2015年   557篇
  2014年   631篇
  2013年   2956篇
  2012年   1165篇
  2011年   1094篇
  2010年   723篇
  2009年   702篇
  2008年   1000篇
  2007年   987篇
  2006年   868篇
  2005年   818篇
  2004年   762篇
  2003年   701篇
  2002年   713篇
  2001年   1280篇
  2000年   1239篇
  1999年   909篇
  1998年   414篇
  1996年   377篇
  1992年   899篇
  1991年   842篇
  1990年   857篇
  1989年   738篇
  1988年   735篇
  1987年   702篇
  1986年   736篇
  1985年   802篇
  1984年   605篇
  1983年   557篇
  1982年   378篇
  1981年   369篇
  1979年   714篇
  1978年   455篇
  1977年   446篇
  1976年   418篇
  1975年   592篇
  1974年   676篇
  1973年   717篇
  1972年   624篇
  1971年   586篇
  1970年   581篇
  1969年   564篇
  1968年   753篇
  1967年   666篇
  1966年   599篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
2.
Previous studies showed that random error can explain overconfidence effects typically observed in the literature. One of these studies concluded that, after accounting for random error effects in the data, there is little support for cognitive‐processing biases in confidence elicitation. In this paper, we investigate more closely the random error explanation for overconfidence. We generated data from four models of confidence and then estimated the magnitude of random error in the data. Our results show that, in addition to the true magnitude of random error specified in the simulations, the error estimates are influenced by important cognitive‐processing biases in the confidence elicitation process. We found that random error in the response process can account for the degree of overconfidence found in calibration studies, even when that overconfidence is actually caused by other factors. Thus, the error models say little about whether cognitive biases are present in the confidence elicitation process. Copyright © 2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
3.
4.
5.
There is substantial evidence of detrimental psychological sequelae following disasters, including terrorist attacks. The effect of these events on extreme responses such as suicide, however, is unclear. We tested competing hypotheses about such effects by employing autoregressive integrated moving average techniques to model the impact of September 11 and the Oklahoma City bombing on monthly suicide counts at the local, state, and national level. Unlike prior studies that provided conflicting evidence, rigorous time series techniques revealed no support for an increase or decrease in suicides following these events. We conclude that while terrorist attacks produce subsequent psychological morbidity and may affect self and collective efficacy well beyond their immediate impact, these effects are not strong enough to influence levels of suicide mortality.  相似文献   
6.
7.
8.
9.
10.
Using a Piagétan perspective, this study investigated the ways in which elementary school children perceive changes in the size of a televised image (in this case, a candy bar). The findings suggest that younger children perceive changes in image size from a medium shot to a close-up as changes in the object itself. Children's responses to changes in the televised image parallel their responses to traditional conservation tasks, but conservation of televised images occurs at a later age. In addition, children appear to use different cognitive skills to interpret how a zoom or a cut transforms the size of a televised image. When a zoom is used, children more readily perceive the object as “growing larger.”  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号