全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1134篇 |
免费 | 10篇 |
专业分类
1144篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 13篇 |
2019年 | 19篇 |
2018年 | 27篇 |
2017年 | 33篇 |
2016年 | 33篇 |
2015年 | 19篇 |
2014年 | 21篇 |
2013年 | 117篇 |
2012年 | 52篇 |
2011年 | 41篇 |
2010年 | 33篇 |
2009年 | 24篇 |
2008年 | 47篇 |
2007年 | 47篇 |
2006年 | 39篇 |
2005年 | 34篇 |
2004年 | 34篇 |
2003年 | 38篇 |
2002年 | 29篇 |
2001年 | 17篇 |
2000年 | 18篇 |
1999年 | 24篇 |
1998年 | 13篇 |
1997年 | 16篇 |
1996年 | 11篇 |
1995年 | 14篇 |
1994年 | 9篇 |
1992年 | 12篇 |
1991年 | 11篇 |
1990年 | 13篇 |
1989年 | 11篇 |
1988年 | 13篇 |
1987年 | 12篇 |
1986年 | 12篇 |
1985年 | 16篇 |
1984年 | 18篇 |
1983年 | 16篇 |
1982年 | 9篇 |
1981年 | 8篇 |
1980年 | 12篇 |
1979年 | 19篇 |
1978年 | 16篇 |
1977年 | 15篇 |
1976年 | 10篇 |
1975年 | 10篇 |
1974年 | 9篇 |
1973年 | 7篇 |
1972年 | 10篇 |
1971年 | 8篇 |
1970年 | 9篇 |
排序方式: 共有1144条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
881.
882.
Lindemann O Abolafia JM Pratt J Bekkering H 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》2008,61(4):515-524
The tendency to respond faster with the left hand to relatively small numbers and faster with the right hand to relatively large numbers (spatial numerical association of response codes, SNARC effect) has been interpreted as an automatic association of spatial and numerical information. We investigated in two experiments the impact of task-irrelevant memory representations on this effect. Participants memorized three Arabic digits describing a left-to-right ascending number sequence (e.g., 3-4-5), a descending sequence (e.g., 5-4-3), or a disordered sequence (e.g., 5-3-4) and indicated afterwards the parity status of a centrally presented digit (i.e., 1, 2, 8, or 9) with a left/right keypress response. As indicated by the reaction times, the SNARC effect in the parity task was mediated by the coding requirements of the memory tasks. That is, a SNARC effect was only present after memorizing ascending or disordered number sequences but disappeared after processing descending sequences. Interestingly, the effects of the second task were only present if all sequences within one experimental block had the same type of order. Taken together, our findings are inconsistent with the idea that spatial-numerical associations are the result of an automatic and obligatory cognitive process but do suggest that coding strategies might be responsible for the cognitive link between numbers and space. 相似文献
883.
The three experiments presented in the paper examine visual prior entry (determining which of two stimuli appeared first)
and prior exit (determining which of two stimuli disappeared first) effects with a temporal order judgment (TOJ) task. In
addition to using onset and offset targets, the preceding cues also consisted of either onset or offset stimuli. Typical,
and equivalent, prior entry effects were found when either onset or offset cues preceded the onset targets. Unexpectedly large
prior exit effects where found with the offset targets, with offset cues producing greater capture effects than onset cues.
These findings are consistent with the notion that more attention is allocated to searching the visual field when targets
are more difficult to find. In addition, the results indicate that attentional control settings may be more likely to occur
with more difficult searches. In addition, these findings demonstrate that TOJ tasks provide extremely precise measures of
the allocation of attention and are very sensitive to a range of task manipulations. 相似文献
884.
Henry Jay Becker 《Sex roles》1985,13(3-4):137-148
Education is one institutional arena in which women professionals comprise a majority of adult computer users. Using data from a national survey of schools, it was found that women comprise two-thirds of the primary computer-using teachers (PCUTs) in elementary schools, and the proportion of women in this role in secondary schools is nearly the same as the proportion of women among secondary school teachers as a whole. Elementary schools whose primary computer-using teacher was a woman used their computer more for programming instruction and less as a drill-and-practice tool than where a man was the PCUT. Microcomputers were used for more hours of the day and with more positive consequences at elementary schools whose PCUT was a woman. In contrast, at the secondary level, schools with men PCUTs had more extensive programs of microcomputer use in nearly every aspect measured. 相似文献
885.
Weise DR Pyszczynski T Cox CR Arndt J Greenberg J Solomon S Kosloff S 《Psychological science》2008,19(5):448-455
Research on terror management theory (TMT) indicates that reminders of death affect political attitudes, but political orientation only sometimes moderates these effects. We propose that secure relationships are associated with values of tolerance and compassion, thus orienting people toward liberalism; insecure attachments are associated with more rigid and absolutist values that orient people toward conservatism. Given that attachment relationships become especially active when security needs are heightened, we predicted that mortality salience would be an important factor in understanding the relationship between attachment processes and political orientation. Supporting these ideas, Study 1 showed that after a mortality-salience manipulation, securely attached participants increased their support for a liberal presidential candidate, and less securely attached participants increased their support for a conservative presidential candidate. In Study 2, a secure-relationship prime following a mortality-salience manipulation engendered a less violent approach to the problem of terrorism than did a neutral-relationship prime. We discuss the interaction of TMT processes and individual differences in attachment in shaping political preferences. 相似文献
886.
Why do people need self-esteem? A theoretical and empirical review 总被引:18,自引:0,他引:18
Terror management theory (TMT; J. Greenberg, T. Pyszczynski, & S. Solomon, 1986) posits that people are motivated to pursue positive self-evaluations because self-esteem provides a buffer against the omnipresent potential for anxiety engendered by the uniquely human awareness of mortality. Empirical evidence relevant to the theory is reviewed showing that high levels of self-esteem reduce anxiety and anxiety-related defensive behavior, reminders of one's mortality increase self-esteem striving and defense of self-esteem against threats in a variety of domains, high levels of self-esteem eliminate the effect of reminders of mortality on both self-esteem striving and the accessibility of death-related thoughts, and convincing people of the existence of an afterlife eliminates the effect of mortality salience on self-esteem striving. TMT is compared with other explanations for why people need self-esteem, and a critique of the most prominent of these, sociometer theory, is provided. 相似文献
887.
Davood G. Gozli Stephanie C. Goodhew Joshua B. Moskowitz Jay Pratt 《Psychological research》2013,77(5):528-539
The ideomotor theory of action posits that the cognitive representation of an action includes the learned perceptual effects of the action. Support for this theory has come from studies demonstrating how perceptual features that match the outcome of a response can facilitate selection of that response. We investigated another, complementary implication of ideomotor theory: would a bias toward selecting a response result in a perceptual bias toward the known effect of the response? In other words, would an action tendency direct attention to the anticipated perceptual features? Through an initial acquisition phase, participants learned that two possible responses (left/right keypress) consistently produced two distinct colors. Next, in a test phase, we manipulated response bias at the beginning of each trial, using an uninformative spatial prime presented at the left or right periphery. We then examined the extent to which color transients that either matched or mismatched the induced response bias can orient participants’ visual attention. Results revealed a perceptual bias toward the color effect of the primed response, manifested in a stronger visual orienting toward this color. Thus, biasing response selection can bias perception. These findings extend the scope of the ideomotor theory to visual perceptual processes. 相似文献
888.
What's in a grade? Academic success and political orientation 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Expanding the literature on person-environment fit, the authors argue that political orientation is an important factor in shaping academic success in college. Based on social dominance theory, it was expected that academic disciplines that are more likely to provide students with future access to social and economic power tend to favor individuals who hold attitudes that strengthen the existing societal order. In a longitudinal sample of undergraduate students at a major American university (n=3,890), the authors demonstrated that student grades in these disciplines, but not in other disciplines, are positively related to a precollege measure of conservatism. This association between conservatism is consistent over time and subgroups, thus implicating higher education in the reproduction of social hierarchy. The discussion examines the causal processes underlying the relationship between political orientation and academic success in college. 相似文献
889.
In the present study, we explore agreement between company versus self-reported reasons for job loss and the degree of perceived
control employees report over the job loss, looking to see if either job loss type or perceived control is related to subsequent
levels of mastery and depression. Two thousand two hundred seventy-nine of 3,700 (61%) employees responded to a survey at
Time One; of these participants, 310 later lost their jobs and were mailed a Time Two survey. Comparison of self- versus company-reported
reasons for the job loss from the 171 usable surveys (55% response rate) revealed relatively good agreement. Roughly one third
of the former employees, however, categorized the job loss in more than one way. There was also a very strong tendency for
employees to report that they had very high control over the decision to leave the company, and nearly all employees reported
improvements to their levels of mastery and depression; exceptions to this pattern were observed for those losing their jobs
due to “rule violation” or “involuntary layoff.” Control over the job loss was related to Time Two levels of mastery and depression,
but only weakly related to changes in these outcomes moving from Time One to Time Two. 相似文献
890.
Karen L. Bierman John D. Cole Kenneth A. Dodge Mark T. Greenberg John E. Lochman Robert J. McMahon 《American journal of community psychology》1997,25(4):493-514
Childhood conduct problems are predictive of a number of serious long-term difficulties (e.g., school failure, delinquent behavior, and mental health problems), making the design of effective prevention programs a priority. The Fast Track Program is a demonstration project currently underway in four demographically diverse areas of the United States, testing the feasibility and effectiveness of a comprehensive, multicomponent prevention program targeting children at risk for conduct disorders. This paper describes some lessons learned about the implementation of this program in a rural area. Although there are many areas of commonality in terms of program needs, program design, and implementation issues in rural and urban sites, rural areas differ from urban areas along the dimensions of geographical dispersion and regionalism, and community stability and insularity. Rural programs must cover a broad geographical area and must be sensitive to the multiple, small and regional communities that constitute their service area. Small schools, homogeneous populations, traditional values, limited recreational, educational and mental health services, and politically conservative climates are all more likely to emerge as characteristics of rural rather than urban sites (Sherman, 1992). These characteristics may both pose particular challenges to the implementation of prevention programs in rural areas, as well as offer particular benefits. Three aspects of program implementation are described in detail: (a) community entry and program initiation in rural areas, (b) the adaptation of program components and service delivery to meet the needs of rural families and schools, and (c) issues in administrative organization of a broadly dispersed tricounty rural prevention program. 相似文献