首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   8654篇
  免费   407篇
  国内免费   3篇
  2023年   46篇
  2021年   104篇
  2020年   178篇
  2019年   157篇
  2018年   242篇
  2017年   236篇
  2016年   288篇
  2015年   204篇
  2014年   252篇
  2013年   863篇
  2012年   430篇
  2011年   451篇
  2010年   275篇
  2009年   268篇
  2008年   400篇
  2007年   395篇
  2006年   332篇
  2005年   345篇
  2004年   314篇
  2003年   294篇
  2002年   293篇
  2001年   150篇
  2000年   106篇
  1999年   139篇
  1998年   115篇
  1997年   112篇
  1996年   96篇
  1995年   82篇
  1994年   80篇
  1993年   80篇
  1992年   92篇
  1991年   84篇
  1990年   73篇
  1989年   56篇
  1988年   56篇
  1987年   69篇
  1986年   67篇
  1985年   74篇
  1984年   72篇
  1983年   74篇
  1982年   67篇
  1981年   80篇
  1980年   74篇
  1979年   63篇
  1978年   70篇
  1977年   74篇
  1976年   66篇
  1975年   57篇
  1974年   61篇
  1968年   42篇
排序方式: 共有9064条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
11.
Various lines of research have recently reported links between the ability to recall past and imagine future episodes. Here I report evidence that such links exist in development. Eighty‐two children were asked to report what they did yesterday and what they are going to do tomorrow. Children who could answer one were also more likely to answer the other question. Furthermore, there was an association between the quantity of responses for past and future questions. This correlation continued to be significant even when controlling for children's capacity to answer similar questions that did not have a temporal, episodic component (e.g., ‘tell me all the things that you can think of that are red’). Together, these results are in line with the growing evidence for fundamental links in the neuro‐cognitive resources involved in thinking about past and future events.  相似文献   
12.
13.
ABSTRACT The central claim is that Hornsby's argument that semantic knowledge is practical knowledge is based upon a false premise. I argue, contra Hornsby, that speakers do not voice their thoughts directly. Rather, our actions of voicing our thoughts are justified by decisions we make (albeit rapidly) about what words to use. Along the way, I raise doubts about other aspects of the thesis that semantic knowledge is practical knowledge.  相似文献   
14.
15.
When forecasting future outcomes, people tend to believe that the outcomes they want to happen are also likely to happen. Despite numerous attempts, few systematic factors have been identified that consistently and robustly reduce wishful thinking (WT) effects. Using elections and sporting event outcomes as contexts, three experiments examined whether taking the perspective of a political rival or opposing fan reduced WT effects. We also examined whether making deliberative (vs. intuitive-based) forecasts was associated with lower WT effects. Online adult samples of U.S. citizens from Mechanical Turk and U.S. college students provided their preferences and forecasts for the U.S. presidential election (Experiments 1 and 2) and a sports competition outcome (Experiment 3). Critically, some participants received perspective taking prompts immediately before providing forecasts. First, results revealed reductions in WT effects when participants engaged in perspective taking. Interestingly, this effect only emerged when intuitive-based forecasts were made first (Experiment 3). Second, intuitive-based forecasts revealed stronger evidence of WT effects. Finally, we found that perspective taking and forming forecasts deliberately promoted a shift in focus away from preferences and toward a consideration of the relative strengths and weaknesses of the entities (i.e., candidates and teams). Theoretical implications for understanding WT effects and applied implications for developing interventions are discussed.  相似文献   
16.
17.
18.
19.
20.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号