全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2066篇 |
免费 | 36篇 |
专业分类
2102篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 8篇 |
2023年 | 13篇 |
2022年 | 16篇 |
2021年 | 25篇 |
2020年 | 62篇 |
2019年 | 53篇 |
2018年 | 75篇 |
2017年 | 79篇 |
2016年 | 83篇 |
2015年 | 57篇 |
2014年 | 68篇 |
2013年 | 222篇 |
2012年 | 104篇 |
2011年 | 125篇 |
2010年 | 62篇 |
2009年 | 64篇 |
2008年 | 109篇 |
2007年 | 103篇 |
2006年 | 89篇 |
2005年 | 82篇 |
2004年 | 65篇 |
2003年 | 66篇 |
2002年 | 62篇 |
2001年 | 24篇 |
2000年 | 33篇 |
1999年 | 29篇 |
1998年 | 15篇 |
1997年 | 13篇 |
1996年 | 13篇 |
1994年 | 11篇 |
1992年 | 8篇 |
1991年 | 12篇 |
1990年 | 14篇 |
1989年 | 8篇 |
1988年 | 7篇 |
1987年 | 27篇 |
1986年 | 11篇 |
1985年 | 8篇 |
1984年 | 9篇 |
1983年 | 12篇 |
1982年 | 9篇 |
1981年 | 11篇 |
1980年 | 7篇 |
1979年 | 14篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1977年 | 7篇 |
1976年 | 9篇 |
1975年 | 14篇 |
1974年 | 7篇 |
1967年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有2102条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
911.
Treiman R Kessler B Zevin JD Bick S Davis M 《Journal of experimental child psychology》2006,93(1):1-24
When college students pronounce nonwords, their vowel pronunciations may be affected not only by the consonant that follows the vowel, the coda, but also by the preceding consonant, the onset. We presented the nonwords used by Treiman and colleagues in their 2003 study to a total of 94 first graders, third graders, fifth graders, and high school students to determine when these context influences emerge. According to some theories of reading development, early decoding is characterized by context-free links from graphemes to phonemes. However, we found that even children reading at the first-grade level (6-year-olds) were influenced to some extent by a vowel's context. The effect of context on vowel pronunciation increased in strength up to around the fifth-grade reading level (8- and 9-year-olds), and sensitivity to coda-to-vowel associations emerged no earlier than did sensitivity to onset-to-vowel associations. A connectionist model of reading reproduced this general pattern of increasing context effects as a function of training. 相似文献
912.
Much research emphasizes heuristic use of stereotypes, though stereotypes have long been considered as capable of influencing more thoughtful processing of social information. Direct comparisons between thoughtful and non-thoughtful stereotyping are lacking in the literature. Recent research in attitude change emphasizes the different consequences of judgments arising from relatively thoughtful versus non-thoughtful processes. Therefore, increased thought could not only fail to decrease stereotyping but might also create stereotypic perceptions that are more likely to have lasting impact. The current studies demonstrate thoughtful and non-thoughtful stereotyping within the same setting. More thoughtful stereotyping is more resistant to future attempts at change and to warnings of possible bias. Implications are discussed for the typical research questions asked after observing stereotypic judgements. 相似文献
913.
In two experiments, we explored the role of foreknowledge on visual search for targets defined along the size continuum. Targets were of large, medium, or small size and of high or low similarity relative to the distractors. In Experiment 1, we compared search for known and unknown singleton feature targets as a function of their size and similarity to the distractors. When distractor similarity was high, target foreknowledge benefited targets at the end of the size continuum (i.e., large and small) relatively more than targets of medium size. In Experiment 2, participants were given foreknowledge of what the target was not The beneficial effect of foreknowledge for endpoint targets was reduced. The data indicate the role of top-down templates in search, which can be "tuned" more effectively for targets at the ends of feature dimensions. 相似文献
914.
This study examined consistency of performance, or intraindividual variability, in older adults' performance on 3 measures of cognitive functioning: inductive reasoning, memory, and perceptual speed. Theoretical speculation has suggested that such intraindividual variability may signal underlying vulnerability or neurologic compromise. Thirty-six participants aged 60 and older completed self-administered cognitive assessments twice a day for 60 consecutive days. Intraindividual variability was not strongly correlated among the 3 cognitive measures, but, over the course of the study, intraindividual variability was strongly intercorrelated within a task. Higher average performance on a measure was associated with greater performance variability, and follow-up analyses revealed that a higher level of intraindividual variability is positively associated with the magnitude of a person's practice-related gain on a particular measure. The authors argue that both adaptive (practice-related) and maladaptive (inconsistency-related) intraindividual variability may exist within the same individuals over time. 相似文献
915.
Jason?D.?BrownEmail author Sabrina?Moraes Janet?Mayhew 《Journal of child and family studies》2005,14(3):417-429
We examined the service needs of foster families with children who have disabilities. Foster parents in a large Canadian city were asked “What services or supports would be helpful to you?” The responses to this question were edited for clarity and to eliminate redundancies, and sorted into piles of like statements by a group of 15 foster care professionals. Two types of statistical analysis were applied to the sorting of the statements to describe the relationship between statements and their groupings. The major concepts were identified according to the contents of the cluster and a map was constructed to provide a graphic representation of the conceptualization process. The major services and supports identified in this study were: support in the community, financial support, accommodating school system, good relationships with social workers, responsive professionals, information, comprehensive medical care, services for aboriginal children and families, transitional services, and respite. 相似文献
916.
Keying-related factors in psychological scales are variously interpreted substantively or as products of violations of the assumptions underlying item keying. The present study investigated whether the extremity of the wording of items may contribute to the emergence of item-keying factors in a commonly used psychological scale. Respondents (N = 277) completed the Life Orientation Test (M. F. Scheier & C. S. Carver, 1985) in either its original or modified, more moderately worded form. Results indicate that the interaction of item extremity and item keying significantly affected subscale means and, more important, that the more moderately worded scale was substantially more unidimensional. Results are explained partially through the association of lesser and greater extremity with the tendency for some respondents to agree or disagree with items irrespective of keying direction. These results, although demonstrated in only 1 scale, have potential relevance to any scale comprising positive and negative items. 相似文献
917.
Paul?Henry?LysakerEmail author Jason?K.?Johannesen John?Timothy?Lysaker 《Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences》2005,4(3):335-352
Many with schizophrenia find social interactions a profound and terrifying threat to their sense of self. To better understand
this we draw upon dialogical models of the self that suggest that those with schizophrenia have difficulty sustaining dialogues
among diverse aspects of self. Because interpersonal exchanges solicit and evoke movement among diverse aspects of self, many
with schizophrenia may consequently find those exchanges overwhelming, resulting in despair, the sensation of fusion with
another, and/or self-dissolution. In short, compromised dialogical capacities may be a contributing factor to social dysfunction
in schizophrenia. 相似文献
918.
Jason Zanes Scott Ross Rudolph Hatfield Bruce Houtler Douglas Whitman 《Personality and individual differences》1998,24(6):879-881
One-hundred and fifteen undergraduates were repeatedly administered a battery measuring schizotypal personality type and creativity (remote associations). Principal Components Analysis found a two-factor model to be most parsimonious. The pattern of results suggests that magical ideation and perceptual aberration items are represented by one factor and suspiciousness by the second. Analysis of consistent versus inconsistent high scorers on the schizoptypia measure indicated that inconsistent high scorers on the suspiciousness factor scored significantly higher than consistent high scorers on the creativity measure. 相似文献
919.
Leonard Jason William Billows Daniel Schnopp-Wyatt Caroline King 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1996,29(3):333-344
The majority of adolescent smokers are able to purchase cigarettes even though laws prohibit the sale of cigarettes to minors (Radecki & Zdunich, 1993). The present study focused on merchant licensing, civil penalties, and monitoring of merchant behavior. Several different schedules of enforcement in the city of Chicago were evaluated to determine the optimal schedules to reduce the sale of cigarettes to minors in a major metropolitan area. Schedules of 2, 4, and 6 months were effective in reducing illegal sales, from 86% to 19%, 87% to 34%, and 87% to 42%, respectively. In a control condition, illegal sales remained high (approximately 84%). Cigarette control laws that regularly enforce civil penalties for tobacco sales violations can successfully reduce minors' access to cigarettes. 相似文献
920.
DISSOCIATION OF STORAGE AND REHEARSAL IN VERBAL WORKING MEMORY: 总被引:30,自引:0,他引:30
Edward Awh John Jonides Edward E. Smith Eric H. Schumacher Robert A. Koeppe Stewart Katz 《Psychological science》1996,7(1):25-31
Abstract— Current cognitive models of verbal working memory include two components a phonological store and a rehearsal mechanism that refreshes the contents of this store We present research using positron emission tomography (PET) to provide further evidence for this functional division In Experiment I, subjects performed a variant of Sternberg's (1966) item recognition task Experiment 2 used a continuous memory task with control conditions designed to separate the brain regions underlying storage and rehearsal The results show that independent brain regions mediate storage and rehearsal In Experiment 3, a dual-task procedure supported the assumption that these memory tasks elicited a rehearsal strategy 相似文献