全文获取类型
收费全文 | 602篇 |
免费 | 10篇 |
专业分类
612篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 19篇 |
2023年 | 19篇 |
2022年 | 15篇 |
2021年 | 22篇 |
2020年 | 37篇 |
2019年 | 41篇 |
2018年 | 48篇 |
2017年 | 43篇 |
2016年 | 31篇 |
2015年 | 24篇 |
2014年 | 16篇 |
2013年 | 78篇 |
2012年 | 20篇 |
2011年 | 34篇 |
2010年 | 19篇 |
2009年 | 18篇 |
2008年 | 25篇 |
2007年 | 18篇 |
2006年 | 21篇 |
2005年 | 19篇 |
2004年 | 9篇 |
2003年 | 8篇 |
2002年 | 10篇 |
2001年 | 3篇 |
2000年 | 5篇 |
1999年 | 2篇 |
1998年 | 1篇 |
1994年 | 1篇 |
1993年 | 2篇 |
1992年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有612条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
361.
How to achieve in elite training centers without burning out? An achievement goal theory perspective
Sandrine Isoard-Gautheur Emma Guillet-Descas Joan L. Duda 《Psychology of sport and exercise》2013,14(1):72-83
In training centers, the demonstration of high competence is essential and there is considerable emphasis placed on sporting achievement. Athlete burnout can be a consequence of such pressures and expectations. More information is needed regarding the social, environmental and individual differences in achievement-related characteristics which are relevant to the occurrence of burnout in this context.ObjectivesTo examine the relationships among the coach-created climate, perceived competence, achievement goals and burnout in elite adolescent-age athletes.DesignProspective six-month-follow-up.MethodsData were collected from a sample of 309 young French handball players participating in elite training centers. Cluster analysis and structural equation modeling procedures were employed to evaluate the hypotheses.ResultsComprised of differential scores on the dimensions of burnout, four distinct clusters were identified. Athletes in these cluster groups varied in perceptions of the motivational climate, goal orientations, and perceived competence. The structural model regarding the hypothesized relationships between perceived coach climate, perceived competence, achievement goals and athlete burnout, offered good fit to the data.ConclusionsFindings indicated that young talented athletes perceiving an ego-involving climate and emphasizing mastery avoidance goals at the beginning of the season had a higher risk of experiencing burnout symptoms at the season's end. In contrast, players perceiving a high task-involving climate and emphasizing mastery approach goals at the beginning of the season had lower burnout scores when the season concluded. Moreover, players with high feelings of competence, who also report higher scores on performance approach and avoidance goals, higher scores on mastery approach goals and lower scores on mastery avoidance goals, are less likely to experience burnout. 相似文献
362.
Emma Ferneyhough Damian A. Stanley Elizabeth A. Phelps Marisa Carrasco 《Psychonomic bulletin & review》2010,17(4):529-535
Faces are unlike other visual objects we encounter, in that they alert us to potentially relevant social information. Both
face processing and spatial attention are dominant in the right hemisphere of the human brain, with a stronger lateralization
in right- than in left-handers. Here, we demonstrate behavioral evidence for an effect of handedness on performance in tasks
using faces to direct attention. Nonpredictive, peripheral cues (faces or dots) directed exogenous attention to contrast-varying
stimuli (Gabor patches)—a tilted target, a vertical distractor, or both; observers made orientation discriminations on the
target stimuli. Whereas cuing with dots increased contrast sensitivity in both groups, cuing with faces increased contrast
sensitivity in right- but not in left-handers, for whom opposite hemifield effects resulted in no net increase. Our results
reveal that attention modulation by face cues critically depends on handedness and visual hemifield. These previously unreported
interactions suggest that such lateralized systems may be functionally connected. 相似文献
363.
Emma H. Moscardini;Eathan A. Breaux;D. Nicolas Oakey-Frost;Raymond P. Tucker; 《Suicide & life-threatening behavior》2024,54(3):425-436
According to the integrated motivational-volitional model of suicide, feelings of entrapment are related to the emergence of suicidal ideation (SI). However, this relation can be moderated by certain protective factors. 相似文献
364.
Jessica L. Gerner;Raymond P. Tucker;Emma H. Moscardini;Courtney L. Bagge;Mark A. Reger; 《Suicide & life-threatening behavior》2024,54(3):501-514
The Virtual Hope Box (VHB) mobile application is an adapted version of the conventional hope box intervention that is used in several evidence-based treatments for suicide behaviors. The VHB is an award-winning app developed by a collaboration between the Departments of Defense and Veterans Affairs. Multiple studies have assessed the utility and effectiveness of the VHB for use in suicide prevention, but no reviews of the literature have been conducted. 相似文献
365.
In chimpanzees, it is only in the restricted context of tool use that manual and cognitive skills have been described, comparable
to those that gorillas and orang-utans display in obtaining plant foods. We report the complex food preparation skills used
to eat, without tools, the leaves of the tree Broussonettia papyrifera in the Sonso community of chimpanzees at Budongo Forest, Uganda. Able-bodied individuals used multi-stage techniques that
required bimanual role differentiation at several stages, and were hierarchical in organisation. A total repertoire of 14
techniques was found, with strong preference in all individuals for either of two of these; 6 additional techniques were found
when flowers and leaves were eaten together. However, in this community over 20% of individuals suffer from some form of upper-
or lower-limb injury as a result of snares. We investigated the manner of compensation for upper-limb injury. Only the most
severely injured showed reduced feeding efficiency. Injured individuals were found to use the same repertoire of techniques
as able-bodied chimpanzees. We found no evidence to suggest that injured individuals were able to develop wholly novel techniques
optimal for their specific injuries, although shifts in preference for particular techniques did occur. Rather, injured individuals
used novel ways of achieving particular steps in the process; by "working around" their impairments; in this way, they managed
to use the same techniques as the able-bodied. Since snare injuries generally befall young animals, these results suggest
that chimpanzees learn techniques partly through observational learning (of, necessarily, able-bodied individuals).
Accepted after revision: 8 February 2001
Electronic Publication 相似文献
366.
Osvath and Osvath (Anim Cogn 11: 661–674, 2008) report innovative studies with two chimpanzees and one orangutan that suggest
some capacity to select and keep a tool for use about an hour later. This is a welcome contribution to a small, but rapidly
growing, field. Here we point out some of the weaknesses in the current data and caution the interpretation the authors advance.
It is not clear to what extent the apes really engaged in any foresight in these studies.
This is an invited response to Osvath and Osvath (2008). 相似文献
367.
Tinsley CJ Fontaine-Palmer NS Vincent M Endean EP Aggleton JP Brown MW Warburton EC 《Learning & memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.)》2011,18(7):484-492
The roles of muscarinic and nicotinic cholinergic receptors in perirhinal cortex in object recognition memory were compared. Rats' discrimination of a novel object preference test (NOP) test was measured after either systemic or local infusion into the perirhinal cortex of the nicotinic receptor antagonist methyllycaconitine (MLA), which targets alpha-7 (α7) amongst other nicotinic receptors or the muscarinic receptor antagonists scopolamine, AFDX-384, and pirenzepine. Methyllycaconitine administered systemically or intraperirhinally before acquisition impaired recognition memory tested after a 24-h, but not a 20-min delay. In contrast, all three muscarinic antagonists produced a similar, unusual pattern of impairment with amnesia after a 20-min delay, but remembrance after a 24-h delay. Thus, the amnesic effects of nicotinic and muscarinic antagonism were doubly dissociated across the 20-min and 24-h delays. The same pattern of shorter-term but not longer-term memory impairment was found for scopolamine whether the object preference test was carried out in a square arena or a Y-maze and whether rats of the Dark Agouti or Lister-hooded strains were used. Coinfusion of MLA and either scopolamine or AFDX-384 produced an impairment profile matching that for MLA. Hence, the antagonists did not act additively when coadministered. These findings establish an important role in recognition memory for both nicotinic and muscarinic cholinergic receptors in perirhinal cortex, and provide a challenge to simple ideas about the role of cholinergic processes in recognition memory: The effects of muscarinic and nicotinic antagonism are neither independent nor additive. 相似文献
368.
Jon Driver IV Greg Davis Paola Ricciardelli Polly Kidd Emma Maxwell Simon Baron-Cohen 《Visual cognition》2013,21(5):509-540
This paper seeks to bring together two previously separate research traditions: research on spatial orienting within the visual cueing paradigm and research into social cognition, addressing our tendency to attend in the direction that another person looks. Cueing methodologies from mainstream attention research were adapted to test the automaticity of orienting in the direction of seen gaze. Three studies manipulated the direction of gaze in a computerized face, which appeared centrally in a frontal view during a peripheral letter-discrimination task. Experiments 1 and 2 found faster discrimination of peripheral target letters on the side the computerized face gazed towards, even though the seen gaze did not predict target side, and despite participants being asked to ignore the face. This suggests reflexive covert and/or overt orienting in the direction of seen gaze, arising even when the observer has no motivation to orient in this way. Experiment 3 found faster letter discrimination on the side the computerized face gazed towards even when participants knew that target letters were four times as likely on the opposite side. This suggests that orienting can arise in the direction of seen gaze even when counter to intentions. The experiments illustrate that methods from mainstream attention research can be usefully applied to social cognition, and that studies of spatial attention may profit from considering its social function. 相似文献
369.
Studies of bee cognition frequently use two-dimensional stimuli referred to as floral patterns, and yet how bees perceive pictorial representations is not known. An investigation of bumblebee (Bombus impatiens) picture–object correspondence was undertaken according to the theory of Fagot et al. (Picture perception in animals. Psychology Press Ltd, East Sussex, pp 295–320, 2000) that pictures and objects may be confused, perceived as independent or equivalent. In three experiments, bumblebees were given discrimination training and unrewarded testing in a radial maze. In the first experiment, preferences between artificial flowers and photographs of those flowers revealed a significant learned preference for the stimulus rewarded during training: no confusion following training. In the second experiment, bees did transfer learning from artificial flowers to photographs: some commonality between an object and photograph was perceived. In the third experiment, bees spontaneously generalized a learned preference for one artificial flower to its silhouette, but only for one of two flowers used in training. No generalization was obtained to drawn images. Some transfer between image and object is exhibited, likely by low-level feature matching, but transfer is poor with degraded images, cautioning against extrapolation of picture-based responding to natural correspondents. Despite evidence that bees exhibit some transfer while retaining discrimination, it is likely that the observed response is due to generalization more akin to confusion than true equivalence. Furthermore, although 2D patterning cues (line, edge and shade) provide discriminable cues for bees between 2D stimuli, it is not here supported that such features are perceived as equivalent to the intended floral structures. 相似文献
370.
Emma Bedor 《Sexuality & culture》2016,20(1):38-55
Released to an American market in 2013 the pharmaceutical drug Osphena was heralded as an innovative, hormone-free therapeutic option to cure two dysfunctions associated with menopausal women’s bodies: vaginal atrophy and dyspareunia. Yet a critical examination of the drug, its advertising, and associated discourses presents an opportunity to explore not only the medicalization of aging but also direct-to-consumer advertising’s role in perpetuating ideologies concerned with normalcy, normal bodies, normal sex, and normal gender performance. This project undertakes a critical feminist analysis of Osphena’s advertising campaign and the public’s response, incorporating Foucaultian theory and a bioethical perspective, and ultimately re-contextualizing them within medical discourses that highlight the pharmaceutical benefits associated with widespread adoption of the “deficiency” perspective that the drug perpetuates. I claim that the Osphena campaign is a clear contemporary illustration that the age-old rhetoric of women’s bodies as requiring medical intervention to resist aging is far from passé, and suggest that images and rhetoric of empowerment would be more readily accepted without relying upon dated and clichéd depictions of female sexuality. 相似文献