全文获取类型
收费全文 | 392篇 |
免费 | 15篇 |
专业分类
407篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 3篇 |
2023年 | 3篇 |
2021年 | 6篇 |
2020年 | 9篇 |
2019年 | 11篇 |
2018年 | 18篇 |
2017年 | 20篇 |
2016年 | 14篇 |
2015年 | 5篇 |
2014年 | 13篇 |
2013年 | 40篇 |
2012年 | 16篇 |
2011年 | 18篇 |
2010年 | 15篇 |
2009年 | 11篇 |
2008年 | 18篇 |
2007年 | 19篇 |
2006年 | 17篇 |
2005年 | 15篇 |
2004年 | 13篇 |
2003年 | 11篇 |
2002年 | 11篇 |
2001年 | 11篇 |
2000年 | 8篇 |
1999年 | 3篇 |
1998年 | 5篇 |
1997年 | 6篇 |
1996年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 2篇 |
1994年 | 2篇 |
1993年 | 4篇 |
1992年 | 5篇 |
1989年 | 5篇 |
1988年 | 8篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 4篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 3篇 |
1972年 | 1篇 |
1971年 | 1篇 |
1970年 | 2篇 |
1969年 | 1篇 |
1968年 | 3篇 |
1967年 | 3篇 |
1966年 | 2篇 |
1960年 | 1篇 |
1953年 | 2篇 |
1933年 | 1篇 |
1919年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有407条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
281.
Roefs A Huijding J Smulders FT MacLeod CM de Jong PJ Wiers RW Jansen AT 《Psychological bulletin》2011,137(1):149-193
Studies obtaining implicit measures of associations in Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed., Text Revision; American Psychiatric Association, 2000) Axis I psychopathology are organized into three categories: (a) studies comparing groups having a disorder with controls, (b) experimental validity studies, and (c) incremental and predictive validity studies. In the first category, implicit measures of disorder-relevant associations were consistent with explicit beliefs for some disorders (e.g., specific phobia), but for other disorders evidence was either mixed (e.g., panic disorder) or inconsistent with explicit beliefs (e.g., pain disorder). For substance use disorders and overeating, expected positive and unexpected negative associations with craved substances were found consistently. Contrary to expectation, implicit measures of self-esteem were consistently positive for patients with depressive disorder, social phobia, and body dysmorphic disorder. In the second category, short-term manipulations of disorder-relevant states generally affected implicit measures as expected. Therapeutic interventions affected implicit measures for one type of specific phobia, social phobia, and panic disorder, but not for alcohol use disorders or obesity. In the third category, implicit measures had predictive value for certain psychopathological behaviors, sometimes moderated by the availability of cognitive resources (e.g., for alcohol and food, only when cognitive resources were limited). The strengths of implicit measures include (a) converging evidence for dysfunctional beliefs regarding certain disorders and consistent new insights for other disorders and (b) prediction of some psychopathological behaviors that explicit measures cannot explain. Weaknesses include (a) that findings were inconsistent for some disorders, raising doubts about the validity of the measures, and (b) that understanding of the concept "implicit" is incomplete. 相似文献
282.
Pinheiro KA Pinheiro RT Silva RA Coelho FM Quevedo Lde Á Godoy RV Jansen K Lessa Horta B Oses JP 《Infant behavior & development》2011,34(2):371-373
A cohort study was conducted with 397 women randomly selected from the Brazilian National System of Public Health, to describe the prevalence rates of infant sleep problems at 12-month of life, and its association with chronicity and severity of maternal depressive symptoms. Mothers were assessed, first, from the 9th to the 12th week postpartum and then at 12 months after delivery, with EPDS and a self-rating questionnaire regarding babies’ sleep behavior. After controlling for possible confounders, babies whose mothers had severe chronic depressive symptoms were at higher risk for sleep disorder at 12 months of birth. 相似文献
283.
Fritz Allhoff 《Science and engineering ethics》2011,17(1):1-19
This paper explores the relationships that various applied ethics bear to each other, both in particular disciplines and more
generally. The introductory section lays out the challenge of coming up with such an account and, drawing a parallel with
the philosophy of science, offers that applied ethics may either be unified or disunified. The second section develops one
simple account through which applied ethics are unified, vis-à-vis ethical theory. However, this is not taken to be a satisfying
answer, for reasons explained. In the third section, specific applied ethics are explored: biomedical ethics; business ethics;
environmental ethics; and neuroethics. These are chosen not to be comprehensive, but rather for their traditions or other
illustrative purposes. The final section draws together the results of the preceding analysis and defends a disunity conception
of applied ethics. 相似文献
284.
Relationships of gender, age, and education to leadership styles and leaders’ influence tactics were examined with 56 leaders
and 234 followers from a variety of organizations. Leadership behaviors were measured with the Multi-factor Leadership Questionnaire
(MLQ—rater version). Influence tactics were measured with Yukl’s Influence Behavior Questionnaire (IBQ). Multivariate Analysis
of Variance (MANOVA) was used to test behavioral differences attributed to leaders’ gender, age, and education groups, as
well as the interaction of age and education with gender. Results show that gender produced a small direct effect on leadership
behaviors. The interaction of gender and education produced consistent differences in leadership behaviors. Implications for
future research are provided, and a call for re-analysis of previously published work is advised. 相似文献
285.
Peter Fritz 《No?s (Detroit, Mich.)》2017,51(3):547-564
A new puzzle of modal recombination is presented which relies purely on resources of first‐order modal logic. It shows that naive recombinatorial reasoning, which has previously been shown to be inconsistent with various assumptions concerning propositions, sets and classes, leads to inconsistency by itself. The context sensitivity of modal expressions is suggested as the source of the puzzle, and it is argued that it gives us reason to reconsider the assumption that the notion of metaphysical necessity is in good standing. 相似文献
286.
Two studies tested the hypothesis that the knew-it-all-along effect may be the result of an inferential process. Specifically, that individuals use their feelings and experiences (e.g., "This question seems so familiar to me, surely I would have known the answer!") to infer their judgement. Drawing on subjective feelings such as certainty or perceptual fluency, individuals can use a provided actual value as an informational cue and draw inferences from it. Thus, the occurrence of the knew-it-all-along effect is expected to depend on the experienced feeling of confidence with a question. This feeling may indicate to an individual that he or she did know the answer; a total lack of such a feeling may suggest that he or she never would have known the answer. In the reported studies we both measured feelings of confidence (Study 1) and induced them by manipulating perceptual fluency (Study 2) to show that the knew-it-all-along effect proves to be a phenomenon of inferences based on these experienced feelings. Participants experiencing high confidence or high perceptual fluency more strongly assimilated their judgements to the provided values, than did participants experiencing low confidence or low perceptual fluency. 相似文献
287.
288.
289.
290.
Peter G. Renden Annemarie Landman Suzanne F. Geerts Sander E.M. Jansen Gert S. Faber Geert J.P. Savelsbergh 《Anxiety, stress, and coping》2014,27(1):100-112
We investigated the effects of anxiety on the execution of police officers' arrest and self-defense skills. Police officers (n=13) performed three tasks in which they kicked, blocked, or restrained an opponent who attacked them with a rubber knife (low anxiety, LA) or a shock knife (high anxiety, HA) in a within-subject design. We analyzed performance (on a 5-point Likert scale), movement times, posture, and movement velocity and acceleration. Results revealed that performance was worse in the HA compared to the LA condition. Furthermore, analysis of full-body movement showed that under increased anxiety, police officers' performance contained characteristics of avoidance behavior, such as faster reactions (to reduce the time being exposed to the threat), leaning further backward (kick), and ducking down (block). In line with recent theoretical developments, it appears that under increased anxiety, police officers were less able to inhibit stimulus-driven processing (fear of getting hit) and enforce goal-directed processing (perform the skill as well as possible) leading to avoidance behavior and a decrease in performance. 相似文献